U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • PMC10204943

Logo of plosone

Laughter as medicine: A systematic review and meta-analysis of interventional studies evaluating the impact of spontaneous laughter on cortisol levels

Caroline kaercher kramer.

1 Division of Endocrinology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada

2 Leadership Sinai Centre for Diabetes, Mount Sinai Hospital, Toronto, Canada

3 Lunenfeld-Tanenbaum Research Institute, Mount Sinai Hospital, Toronto, Canada

Cristiane Bauermann Leitao

4 Division of Endocrinology, Hospital de Clínicas de Porto Alegre, Porto Alegre, Brazil

5 Post-graduate Program in Medical Sciences: Endocrinology, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Brazil

Associated Data

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Laughter as an expression of humor has been recognized as good medicine for centuries. The health benefits of humor-induced well-being remain unclear and thus we conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of interventional studies to evaluate the impact of spontaneous laughter on stress response as measured by cortisol levels.

Systematic review and meta-analysis.

Data sources

MEDLINE/PubMed, EMBASE, PsycINFO, Scopus, and Clinicaltrials.gov.

Eligibility criteria

Interventional studies, which could be either randomized placebo-controlled trials (RCTs) or quasi-experimental studies, conducted in adults that compared any spontaneous laughter intervention to a controlled setting and reported changes in cortisol levels were selected.

Data extraction and synthesis

We examined the impact of laughter on percentage change in cortisol levels by calculating pooled estimates of the absolute differences between arithmetic means before and after interventions as compared to control using random-effects model.

Eight studies (315 participants; mean age 38.6) met our inclusion criteria; four were RCTs and four were quasi-experiment studies. Five studies evaluated the impact of watching a humor/comedy video, two studies evaluating laughter sessions administered by a trained laughter therapist, and one study evaluating a self-administered laughter program. Pooling these data showed a significant reduction in cortisol levels by 31.9% (95%CI -47.7% to -16.3%) induced by laughter intervention compared to control group with no evidence of publication bias (P = 0.66). Sensitivity analyses demonstrated that even a single laughter session induced a significant reduction of 36.7% in cortisol (95%CI -52.5% to -20.8%). In addition, analyses including the four RCTs reinforced these results by demonstrating a significant reduction in cortisol levels promoted by laughter as compared to the placebo arm [-37.2% (95%CI -56.3% to -18.1%)].

Conclusions

Current evidence demonstrates that spontaneous laughter is associated with greater reduction in cortisol levels as compared with usual activities, suggesting laughter as a potential adjunctive medical therapy to improve well-being.

Trial registration

Registration number: CRD42021267972 .

Introduction

Laughter as an expression of humor has been recognized as a good medicine for centuries [ 1 ], a concept consistent with human neurodevelopment. Specifically, the capacity for laughter in humans precedes the neural development of speech [ 2 ] with neuroimaging studies suggesting a unique neural pathway for spontaneous laughter (i.e. genuine laughter) [ 3 ] that is intuitive and subcortical. Documented in Charles Darwin’s classic work [ 4 ], ancestral forms of play homologous to human laughter have been reported in other animals (dogs, chimps, and even rats) [ 2 , 5 , 6 ] being perceived by Darwin and other authors as a natural response that restores homeostasis across diverse species including homo sapiens .

In 1976, a remarkable publication in The New England Journal of Medicine highlighted a potential therapeutic role for spontaneous laughter [ 7 ]. In that report, the author/patient described his regimen of self-prescribed laughter as an adjunctive therapy that provided him with potent anesthetic effect attenuating symptoms caused by ankylosing spondylitis: “I made the joyous discovery that 10 minutes of genuine belly laughter had an anesthetic effect and would give me at least two hours of pain-free sleep” [ 7 ]. Subsequently, recent research has linked laughter and humor with increased levels of pain tolerance [ 8 ], and positive impact in overall well-being in diverse medical settings such as oncology [ 9 ], psychiatry [ 10 ], and rehabilitation [ 11 ]. Data from a recent randomized controlled trial (RCT) demonstrated a positive and robust effect of having a humor journal on well-being [ 12 ], suggesting that humorous interventions are simple underutilized strategies that can help with coping in an adverse situation such as the coronavirus pandemic [ 13 ].

The activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) axis and consequent elevation in glucocorticoids is a key physiological response to either physical (i.e. acute and chronic diseases) or psychological stressors (i.e. anticipated recognition of a threat). Coupled with the autonomic nervous system, the HPA axis represents a vital neuroendocrine system that is dynamically regulated in a feedback-loop manner in order to maintain homeostasis of virtually the entire human body [ 14 ]. Despite the long-dated acknowledgment of humor as medicine, the impact of spontaneous laughter on neuroendocrine stress response remain unclear. Previous reports in adults have suggested that spontaneous laughter can impact the HPA axis by reducing cortisol levels [ 15 – 22 ]. However, the small sample sizes preclude definitive conclusions based on these individual studies, suggesting the need for a robust and systematic evaluation of the mechanistic effect of laughter by meta-analysis. Thus, we conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of interventional studies to evaluate the impact of spontaneous laughter on cortisol levels.

This systematic review and meta-analysis is reported in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) Statement and was registered with the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews ( http://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/ ; CRD42021267972) [ 23 ].

Research ethics approval

Not applicable.

Data sources and searches

We selected relevant studies published between 1950 and April 20, 2022. We searched MEDLINE/PubMed, EMBASE, PsycINFO, Scopus, and Clinicaltrials.gov using the following combined text and Medical Subject Heading (MeSH) terms: “laughter”, “laughter therapy”, “humor therapy”, “cortisol”. The complete PubMed search was as follows: ("Laughter"[Mesh] OR "Laughter therapy"[Mesh]) OR ("Laughter"[Text word] OR "Laughter therapy"[Text word]) OR (Humor therapy [Text Word]). The online Table 1 shows the complete search strategies used to search each database or registry. All potentially eligible studies were considered for review, regardless of the primary outcome or language. We also conducted a manual search using references of the included articles published in English.

Study selection

Studies were eligible for inclusion if they: (1) were interventional studies, which could be either RCT or quasi-experimental studies conducted in adults, (2) compared any spontaneous laughter intervention to a controlled setting consisting of either an intervention not associated with humor (i.e. such as reading a book) or usual activities (control group), and (3) reported changes in cortisol levels (serum/plasma or salivary cortisol). Exclusion criteria were as follows: studies that evaluated laughter-inducing therapy as part of physical activity intervention given the impact of exercise itself on stress hormones [ 24 ], studies that did not report a control group, retrospective studies or observational studies, and studies that measured cortisol in unstandardized samples (i.e. breast milk and hair follicle) ( Fig 1 ). If a study reported data on more than one humorous intervention arm, we included the data that fulfilled our inclusion criteria.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0286260.g001.jpg

Intervention investigated and outcome measurements

We evaluated any spontaneous laughter intervention, which included (i) watching a comedy movie, (ii) laughter therapy conducted by a trained laughter therapist consisting of activities that promoted laughter, and (iii) self-administered laughter therapy. The laughter intervention could be applied on only one experimental day or as intermittent therapy sessions over a continuous period of time. The primary outcome was the percentage mean difference in the change in cortisol (serum/plasma or salivary) between baseline and end of intervention between intervention and control groups. These data was calculated based on changes in cortisol in relation to baseline values. The measure of variance was extracted by either absolute values, graphical displays, or estimated according to the Cochrane handbook of systematic reviews or meta-analysis.

Data extraction and quality assessment

Two independent investigators (CKK, CBL) reviewed study titles and abstracts. Studies that satisfied the inclusion criteria were retrieved for full-text assessment. Studies selected for review by both investigators had an agreement value (k) of 97%; disagreements were resolved by further discussion between the investigators. The following data was extracted from each study: study design, total number of participants, duration of intervention, percentage mean changes in cortisol levels. The risk of bias was evaluated according to the revised Cochrane tool for assessing risk of bias in randomized trials (RoB 2) [ 25 ] and the risk of bias in non-randomized studies of interventions (ROBINS-I) tool [ 26 ]. In addition, we evaluated the quality of evidence using the Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation (GRADE) for meta-analyses [ 27 ].

Data synthesis and analysis

We examined the impact of spontaneous laughter on cortisol levels as assessed by percentage mean change in cortisol. We calculated pooled estimates of the absolute differences between arithmetic means before and after interventions as compared to control group using a random-effects model (inverse-variance DerSimonian-Laird method). The I 2 value was used to evaluate the magnitude of heterogeneity between studies, with values greater than 50% indicating moderate-to-high heterogeneity [ 28 ]. We performed meta-regression analyses to assess whether the duration of laughter-inducing intervention impacted the stress response measured by changes in cortisol levels. In addition, we performed the following sensitivity analyses: (i) including studies that evaluated the impact of a single session of humorous intervention on cortisol levels, (ii) stratifying the studies by laughter intervention (watching comedy movie and laughter therapy), (iii) stratifying the studies by cortisol assay (salivary and serum cortisol), and (iv) including only RCTs. The possibility of publication bias was evaluated using a funnel plot of effect size against the standard error for each trial. Funnel plot asymmetry was evaluated by Begg’s and Egger’s tests, with significant publication bias defined as a P value <0.1 [ 29 ]. All analyses were performed using Stata 14.0 (Stata Corp, College Station, Texas).

Patient and public involvement

There was no patient involved in this study design and results. However, we hope that with the publication of this report, further studies and implementation of interventions to improve well-being through laughter could benefit patients.

Study characteristics

We identified 3,089 studies, of which 3,070 were excluded on the basis of title and abstract. Nineteen studies were retrieved for detailed assessment, eleven of which excluded ( Fig 1 ). Eight studies (with data from 315 participants) met our inclusion criteria; four were RCTs [ 16 , 17 , 19 , 22 ] and four were quasi-experiment studies [ 15 , 18 , 20 , 21 ] ( Table 1 ).

Included studies were published between 1989 and 2021, five studies evaluated laughter intervention as a single experiment [ 15 – 17 , 19 , 22 ] while three studies evaluated more than one laughter session over 4–6 weeks of time [ 18 , 20 , 21 ]. Patients had a mean age of 38.6 years old (ranging from 24.0 to 68.5 years), and 12.5–62.5% were male with one study conducted only in men [ 15 ] and one only in women [ 20 ]. The population evaluated in these studies included healthy individuals [ 15 , 16 , 19 , 21 , 22 ], participants living with diabetes and obesity [ 17 , 20 ], and patients on hemodialysis [ 18 ]. The humorous intervention adopted in these studies varied, with five studies evaluating the impact of watching a humor/comedy video [ 15 , 16 , 17 , 19 , 22 ], two studies evaluating laughter sessions administered by a trained laughter therapist [ 18 , 20 ], and one study evaluating a self-administered laughter program [ 21 ]. Salivary [ 17 , 19 , 21 , 22 ] and serum [ 15 , 16 , 18 , 20 ] cortisol were assessed at the same time of the day between the study arms, although the time of cortisol collection varied between the studies ( Table 1 ).

The assessment of risk of bias is shown in Fig 2 . The RCTs presented concerns in the overall assessment of bias, mainly due to deviations on the intended intervention as the laughter intervention cannot remain blinded to the participants ( Fig 2A ). In the same way, the quasi-experimental studies had moderate possibility of bias related to possible confounding effect and impossibility of blinded intervention ( Fig 2B ).The dropout rates varied from 0% to 27.5%. Although some studies informed the participants about the possibility of adverse events with the humor intervention, there were no adverse events reported.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0286260.g002.jpg

(Panel A) Risk of bias in randomized trials (RoB 2). (Panel B) Risk of bias in non-randomized studies of interventions (ROBINS-I).

Impact of laughter on cortisol levels

Eight studies assessed the change in cortisol levels between laughter-inducing intervention and control group [ 15 – 22 ]. Pooling the data from these studies (n = 315 participants) showed a significant reduction in cortisol levels by 31.9% (95%CI -47.7% to -16.3%) induced by humorous intervention compared to control, with between-study heterogeneity (I 2 74.4%, P < 0.01) ( Fig 3A ). In this analysis, there was no evidence of publication bias on the Egger test (P = 0.66). In order to evaluate whether the duration of laughter-inducing activity (minutes) impacted the attenuation of stress response measured by reduction in cortisol levels, we performed meta-regression analyses which demonstrated no impact of the duration of spontaneous laughter on the observed reduction in cortisol ( Fig 3B ). To further assess the impact of laughter intervention on cortisol levels, we performed additional analysis including the studies that assessed a single laughter session (ranging from 9 to 60 minutes) [ 15 – 17 , 19 , 22 ]. This analysis demonstrated that even a single laughter session induced a reduction of 36.7% in cortisol (95%CI -52.5% to -20.8%) as compared to control group. In addition, analyses stratified by the type of laughter intervention (watching comedy movie and laughter therapy) demonstrated a reduction in cortisol level regardless of the strategy to induce spontaneous laughter [comedy movie: -36.7% (95%CI -52.5% to -20.8%); laughter therapy: -18.9% (95%CI -34% to -3.2%)].

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0286260.g003.jpg

(Panel A) Meta-analysis of the percentage mean difference in cortisol (%) between laughter interventions and control. CI = confidence interval. (Panel B) Bubble plot with fitted meta-regression line showing relationship between duration of laughter intervention and reduction in cortisol.

Next, sensitivity analyses were performed stratifying by the cortisol assay: (i) serum/plasma cortisol, and (ii) salivary cortisol measurements ( Fig 4 ). As compared to control group, the laughter intervention induced significant decrease in cortisol in studies evaluating serum/plasma cortisol [-22.0% (95%CI -42.3% to -1.77%)] ( Fig 4A ). This effect was even more pronounced when the stress response was assessed by changes in salivary cortisol with a 43.9% reduction in cortisol induced by laughter as compared to control (95%CI -74.1% to -13.7%) ( Fig 4B ). Finally, we repeated the analyses restricting the analysis to the four RCTs [ 16 , 17 , 19 , 22 ] which also demonstrated a significant reduction in cortisol levels promoted by laughter as compared to the placebo arm [-37.2% (95%CI -56.3% to -18.1%)]. The overall GRADE profile from these analyses is moderate/low (not free of biases) for establishing clinical recommendations based on the quality of the data available ( Table 2 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0286260.g004.jpg

(Panel A) Serum cortisol, and (Panel B) Salivary cortisol. CI = confidence interval.

Our results showed that, compared to non-humorous usual activities, interventions that trigger spontaneous laughter induced a significant reduction of ~32% in cortisol levels, suggesting an impact on HPA-axis as a metabolic pathway associated with the stress-relief effect of humor. The positive impact of laughter on buffering cortisol response was already evident after one single laughter session (~37% reduction), being observed regardless of the laughter-inducing strategy (watching comedy movie and laughter therapy) or cortisol assay (salivary and serum cortisol).

Findings in the context of existing studies

Previous studies have demonstrated the potential impact of sense of humor and laughter on cardiovascular health [ 30 – 32 ]. Laughter-inducing activities such as watching a comedy movie has been associated with improvement in endothelial function assessed by brachial artery flow-mediated vasodilation and carotid arterial compliance, with effects that lasted up to 24-hours [ 30 , 31 ]. The cardioprotective effect of positive emotions was also evident in clinical studies. Specifically, in a study of 300 individuals, Clark et al showed that the propensity to laugh under a variety of situations encountered in everyday life had an inverse association with incident coronary heart disease [ 32 ]. This concept is supported by epidemiological data from Women’s Health Initiative (n = 97,253 women) [ 33 ], Nurses’ Health Study (n = 69,744) [ 34 ], and the Veterans Affairs Normative Aging Study (n = 1,429 men) [ 34 ]. These studies demonstrated that optimism, which is closely related to spontaneous laughter (i.e. frequency of laughter is associated with optimism) [ 1 – 4 ], is inversely and independently associated with both cardiovascular and total mortality, being associated with exceptional longevity [ 33 , 34 ]. Our results expand and reinforce the importance of spontaneous laughter in promoting health by demonstrating a significant and objectively-measured reduction on the hallmark adrenal stress-hormone, cortisol, induced by genuine laughter.

A reduction in cortisol levels has also been reported in response to laughter-inducing interventions in children and adolescents [ 35 – 39 ]. A previous meta-analysis of 24 studies (n = 1,612 kids) demonstrated that hospital clowns might contribute to improved psychological well-being and emotional responses in children and adolescents in hospital setting, with four studies reporting reduced levels of salivary cortisol after seeing hospital clowns visits compared with the pre-intervention measurement [ 35 ]. The implementation of humor in clinical care has been more widely established in the pediatric population possibly because the psychological motivations behind genuine laughter in adults is more complex, being influenced by the individual’s cultural background, education, beliefs, and psychological traits [ 1 – 4 , 40 ]. Once developed, however, the interpretation and perception of laughter among adults overcomes cultural and language barriers. In a study evaluating 966 participants from 24 societies, individuals reliably distinguished friends from strangers based on acoustic features of laughter with an accuracy of 53–67% [ 41 ]. These results suggest that, despite the complexity of modern social interactions and diverse cultural backgrounds, laughter is still a nonverbal vocal expression that communicates positive affect and cooperative intent in humans. In parallel to that, laughter can be socially contagious as experiencing other people’s laughter is a powerful stimulus for further laughter [ 42 ] even if the joke lacks comedic component (i.e. a bad joke) [ 43 ].

The blunted response in cortisol induced by spontaneous laughter may have other positive metabolic effects beyond the most evident relaxation and well-being. Hayashi et al, evaluating the impact of laughter induced by a comedy show in individuals with type 2 diabetes not on insulin (16 men and 3 women, age 63.4 years, body mass index 23.5 kg/m 2 , glycated hemoglobin 7.2%) as compared to a usual lecture, observed that the humorous show reduced the postprandial glucose excursion by 2.5 mmol/l [ 44 ]. Another potentially desirable metabolic effect of laughter includes the energy consumption that results from the contraction of facial and abdominal skeletal muscles (diaphragm). In a study evaluating 55 healthy individuals (age 18–34 years; mean body mass index 24.7 kg/m 2 ) using whole-room indirect calorimeter equipped with audio recording system, it was estimated that 15 minutes of genuine laughter can increase energy expenditure by approximately 40 kcal [ 45 ]. Interestingly and relevant to our results, reduced cortisol levels may help hair growth as evidenced by a recent study evaluating the underlying mechanism that modulates hair growth in response to stress. In a study published in Nature , Choi et al have uncovered a cellular and molecular mechanism that links stress hormones produced by adrenal glands to the activation of hair-follicle stem cells through the control of growth arrest-specific 6 (GAS6) expression in dermal papillae [ 46 ].

Implications

The health benefits of self-induced or simulated laughter have also been reported previously [ 47 ] with the most common modality consisting of a combination of yoga breathing techniques with induced laughter exercises, the laughter yoga . We focused this meta-analyses on genuine laughter as (i) the majority of the human humor occurs spontaneously in everyday life [ 13 ], (ii) the combination of laughter with physical activity could impact the interpretation of the cortisol response as physical activity itself can impact the secretion of adrenal hormones [ 22 ], and (iii) previous imaging studies suggest the involvement of different neural pathways in stimulated as compared to spontaneous laughter [ 3 ]. The impact on HPA axis found in our analyses suggests that genuine laughter holds positive effects for overall health as the excessive/prolonged cortisol secretion associated with chronic HPA-axis stimulation has negative implications for both physical and psychological diseases including obesity, depression, and chronic pain [ 48 , 49 ]. RCTs evaluating the long-term impact of genuine laughter are needed to establish whether activities that induce spontaneous laughter could be applied in specific clinical scenarios such as to improve metabolic health, symptoms of anxiety, and coping with chronic stressful situations.

Potential limitations of this study

A possible limitation of our meta-analyses is the heterogeneity amongst the laughter-inducing protocols as the laughter interventions included watching movies and talking to a laughter therapist for different durations of time. In addition, the studies had small samples sizes. However, this is not expected to impact our conclusions as our results were consistent across the diverse protocols and even a single session of laughter was significantly associated with a decreased cortisol response. Another limitation pertains to the inherent bias of quasi-experiment studies design that cannot be ruled out. Nonetheless, the sensitivity analyses including only RCT’s reinforce the main results. In addition, despite the concerns observed in the assessment of risk of bias ( Fig 2 ), the main aspect of concern related to the fact that laughter interventions cannot be applied in a blinded fashion. In this context, the potential impact of social interactions on HPA-axis response to laughter is another relevant aspect that could have impacted the results. Our analyses included studies with laughter intervention applied in both group and individual settings, but the degree of social interaction prior to or combined with the laughter intervention was not described. Another weakness is that the timing of cortisol sampling was inconsistent between studies and could have implications for the degree of change in cortisol induced by laughter. However, the fact that the timing of collection was not different between the study arms and that we assessed percent changes in cortisol may have helped to limit the impact of this possible confounder. Finally, pooling the data on the impact of laughter on cortisol levels in absolute units was not suitable given the diverse biochemistry measurements performed by each study.

In conclusion, our results support the ancient knowledge that spontaneous laughter is in fact good medicine (preventive or therapeutic) being associated with greater reduction in cortisol levels as compared with usual activities. These analyses demonstrated the potential therapeutic role of laughter-inducing interventions as a complementary strategy to improve everyone’s well-being and highlight the need for further research aiming to improve our collective sense of humor.

Supporting information

S1 checklist, acknowledgments.

We dedicate this manuscript to Brazil as humor represents a fundamental characteristic of the Brazilian culture. CBL receives a scholarship from Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq–PQ 1D).

Funding Statement

The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

Data Availability

Introduction to the Special Issue of the Scientific Study of Laughter: Where We Have Been, Current Innovations, and Where We Might Go From Here

  • Published: 16 August 2022
  • Volume 46 , pages 321–326, ( 2022 )

Cite this article

  • Sally D. Farley 1  

2144 Accesses

Explore all metrics

In his pioneering work, The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals , Darwin (1872/1965) argued that laughter was the outward expression of mirth. In the 150 years since the publication of this book, research has revealed that laughter is substantially more complex, nuanced, and socially important than Darwin theorized. The current special issue highlights recent discoveries regarding the nature of laughter and illuminates numerous themes. These include the importance of both representational and constructionist accounts of laughter, the value of studying naturally-occurring laughter “in the wild,” evoked in a variety of contexts (including contexts likely to elicit derisive laughter), and the critical advantage of pre-registration and other open science practices for improving our scientific understanding of laughter.

Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.

A Brief History of the Science of Laughter

In his seminal book The Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals (1872/1965), Darwin wrote that laughter reflected a state of internal happiness and joy and was essentially the audible extension of a smile. Yet in this pioneering work, Darwin ( 1872 /1965) spoke to the importance of context arguing that laughter was not a reflex action. He wrote, “a young child, if tickled by a strange man, would scream from fear” rather than laugh (p. 199). In addition, despite maintaining that laughter was predicated upon being in a pleasant state of mind, Darwin also commented on man’s ability to use laughter strategically, for example, to mask other emotions such as anger or contempt. These early observations were a harbinger for the most significant debate in emotion research in the last 150 years. Specifically, to what extent are nonverbal behaviors universal expressions of underlying emotion, signals for communicative goals, or ambiguous cues that require cultural learning or context to decode? And regarding laughter in particular, is there a difference between laughter evoked in response to genuine mirth and laughter evoked in other situations such as politeness, embarrassment or schadenfreude?

Darwin’s argument that nonverbal expressions signify a readout of emotional experiences gave rise to the most dominant emotion theory in the last 50 years, Basic Emotions Theory (BET)(e.g., Ekman, 1992 ; Ekman & Friesen, 1969 ), which has evolved considerably over this time period (Keltner et al., 2019b ). Specifically, recent advances in BET have shown a marked increase in the number of documented universal emotions (as many as 28, Cowen & Keltner, 2019 ), greater integration with social functionalist accounts, and more attention to the importance of context (Keltner et al., 2019a ). But other theoretical approaches such as the Constructionist View (Barrett & Westlin, 2021 ) and the Behavioral Ecology View of facial displays (BECV; Crivelli & Fridlund, 2019 ) prioritize the role of social goals and context in the meanings we ascribe to nonverbal behavior. These views dispute the universality of emotion and challenge the notion that certain signals (such as facial expressions) convey unique meanings. This may be one of the most significant recurring debates in nonverbal communication and one that is germane to several papers in this special issue (Farley et al., 2022 ; Rychlowska et al., 2022 ; Wood et al., 2022 ).

Another pioneer whose work continues to inspire the scientific study of laughter is the late Richard Provine, to whom we owe a debt of gratitude. Provine’s (2001) painstaking naturalistic observation of more than 1000 laughter episodes contradicted several commonsense notions about laughter. The profoundly social nature of laughter was not terribly surprising, that is, individuals laugh 30 times more frequently in the presence of others than when alone (Provine, 2004 ). However, the findings that laughter is more frequent among speakers than listeners and occurs more in response to nonhumorous comments than humorous comments challenged the putative fixed link between humor and laughter (Provine, 2001 ). Echoing Provine’s work, this special issue showcases two papers that employed behavioral observation (as opposed to self-report) and yielded similarly novel and perplexing findings (Haviva & Starzyk, 2022 ; Mazzocconi & Ginzburg, 2022 ).

In 2005, Gervais and Wilson wrote a compelling treatise on the nature of humor and laughter in The Quarterly Review of Biology arguing for the existence of two unique neural laughter pathways. According to Gervais and Wilson ( 2005 ), “Duchenne” laughter evolved from the play displays of primates and thus reflects genuine positive affect. This type of laughter is spontaneous, involuntary, and emotional, and originates in older subcortical brain regions such as the brain stem and limbic system. In contrast, “Non-Duchenne” laughter is a more recent evolutionary development that can be employed strategically and is produced in neocortical areas. Given the volitional nature of “Non-Duchenne” laughter, it can serve many functions, from polite social glue which facilitates social bonding, to mocking insult which corrodes social relationships. The difficulty in unifying the laughter literature is due in part to its polysemous nature. Although the papers in this special issue do not adopt a uniform laughter classification system, recognition of different laughter types is a theme (Farley et al., 2022 ; Rychlowska et al., 2022 ; Wood et al., 2022 ).

Summary of Articles in This Special Issue

This special issue features significant recent contributions to the scientific study of laughter, ones that tread new terrain and complement the pioneering work discussed above. Despite the ubiquity and importance of laughter, there is very little exploration into the developmental trajectory of laughter in young children. In the first paper of the special issue, Mazzocconi and Ginzburg ( 2022 ) conducted a longitudinal investigation of a small sample of children from 12 to 36 months and noted that 12-month-old infants and their mothers have unique laughter profiles. Over time, however, infant laughter frequency increased and the patterns of child-mother laughter converged, possibly reflecting infants’ greater attunement with their caregivers and increased cognitive abilities (e.g., Theory of Mind). Given that laughter emerges around the third month of life, greatly preceding speech abilities, laughter serves a critical role for appraising and regulating social interaction with caregivers. The second paper in the special issue by Haviva and Starzyk ( 2022 ) examined another scarcely examined population – those at the opposite end of the developmental spectrum—older adults aged 67–95 (Haviva & Starzyk, 2022 ). This rare behavioral observation of laughter, the first to examine correlations between observed laughter frequency and a variety of psychological dimensions, refuted several intuitive notions about laughter. Although there was tremendous variability in laughter frequency among older adults, behavioral observations of laughter markedly exceeded the self-reported frequency reported in previous work, which both contradicts the stereotype that older adults laugh less than younger adults and underscores the unreliability of self-report accounts. Other unexpected findings were that laughter frequency was generally unrelated to dimensions of psychological health/well-being, physical health, and personality and did not differ by gender. Observed laughter frequency was, interestingly, negatively correlated with conscientiousness.

The third paper in the special issue (Smartt et al., 2022 ) picks up the thread of the relationship between laughter and the Big Five personality dimensions and asks the simple but interesting question—does laughter provide reliable cues to someone’s personality? Offering more support for the mysterious nature of laughter, person perception accuracy was generally not enhanced by viewing targets when they were laughing as opposed to when they were not, although observers were more accurate at detecting extraversion in laughing targets. However, people perceived targets to be higher in agreeableness, extraversion, openness to experience and conscientiousness when they were laughing than when they were not. The divergent associations between laughter and conscientiousness in this special issue are worthy of pointing out—Smartt et al. ( 2022 ) found that laughter makes individuals appear more conscientious, but Haviva and Starzyk ( 2022 ) found that frequent laughter was associated with less self-reported conscientiousness.

Because Smartt et al. ( 2022 ) relied upon laughter samples in response to humorous stimuli, these samples are likely to reflect spontaneous “Duchenne” laughter. Clearly, perceptions of laughing individuals experiencing genuine mirth would differ from those laughing at another person’s expense. Wood et al. ( 2022 ) included a negatively-valenced laughter type in their work on the social functional perspective, which describes three functions of smiles and laughs: reward (based on positive affect and desire to elicit positive affect in others), affiliation (based on politeness and desire to convey nonthreat), and dominance (based on a desire to show superiority or other negative affect). Researchers endeavored to determine whether reward, affiliation, and dominance laughs would map on to the same semantic space as their respective categories for smiles. Reward, affiliation, and dominance laugh samples of each type clustered together, as did reward, affiliation, and dominance smiles, but they occupied somewhat different semantic space. This research demonstrated that individuals implicitly judge smiles and laughter along reward, affiliation, and dominance dimensions.

In the fifth paper, Farley et al. ( 2022 ) grounded their work in the dual pathway framework first described by Gervais and Wilson ( 2005 ) and later empirically investigated by Bryant and his colleagues (e.g., Bryant & Aktipis, 2014 ; Bryant et al., 2016 ). Previous research demonstrated that raters, even five-month-old infants, can detect affiliation in short segments of laughter (Bryant & Aktipis, 2014 ; Vouloumanos & Bryant, 2019 ), but this paper showed that raters can accurately differentiate laughter samples from two dyad types both high in affiliation (friends and romantic partners). Farley et al. ( 2022 ) revealed that friend and romantic laughter differed on dimensions of perceived authenticity and vulnerability and argued that friendship laughter has features that are more compatible with the laughter production system that evolved from our primate counterparts. The findings from Farley et al. ( 2022 ) and Wood et al. ( 2022 ) are more supportive of the representational nonverbal perspective in that the laughter samples from these studies were accurately decoded, regardless of context, and in the case of Farley et al. ( 2022 ), across six different cultures.

In contrast, the final paper in this special issue by Rychlowska et al. ( 2022 ) offered compelling evidence as to the importance of context in decoding laughter’s meaning. Rychlowska et al. ( 2022 ) captured laughter evoked in response to three different social situations—amusement, embarrassment, and schadenfreude. They then exposed participants to four conditions with increasing context/informational value (audio only, audio–video with only the laugher visible, audio–video with laugher and conversational partner visible, and audio–video with both partners and precipitating event). Participants in the context-poor conditions did not exceed chance in their ability to identify the social situation that evoked the laughter. This paper complements research conducted by Aviezer and his colleagues ( 2017 ) on emotion detection in the face which posits that contextual information is integral, not peripheral, to the meanings we derive from nonverbal cues (see also Curran et al., 2018 ).

Ideally, the papers in this special issue will inspire further research on the fascinating, and at times counterintuitive, nature of laughter. As a body, they provide several important insights and avenues for future research. First, just like the nature/nurture “debate,” the tension between the representational and constructionist views of laughter perhaps represents a false dichotomy. Research strongly supports the existence of different kinds of laughter that convey unique meanings, but these signals are embedded in a context that colors and potentially overrides these meanings. I echo Keltner et al.’s ( 2019b ) call for more “systematic approaches to understanding how both classes of processes [evolution and culture] shape emotional expression in compelling ways” (p. 199), along with relevant moderators and mediators.

In addition, the dual pathway model posits that volitional laughter and spontaneous laughter differ in substantial ways (Gervais & Wilson, 2005 ). Thus, the methodological approach of sourcing laughter from actors, who will inevitably produce volitional laughter, is problematic in terms of ecological validity. A similar point has been raised about posed facial expressions (Namba et al., 2017 ). A deeper understanding of the richness and nuance of laughter would profit from examining naturally occurring laughter in a variety of contexts.

An additional insight garnered from examining these articles is the unreliability of self-reported laughter frequency, which urges future researchers to return to the laborious method of behavioral observation, a call that is especially important for developmental periods that have yet to be examined. Furthermore, these articles underscore that the field suffers from inconsistent terminology, a barrier to integrating potentially overlapping theoretical frameworks. For example, “Duchenne” laughter may be termed spontaneous, authentic, amusement, or reward laughter in different articles.

One final observation underscored by the present special issue is the immeasurable value gleaned from null findings. The lack of association between laughter frequency and a variety of physical and psychological correlates is both perplexing and counterintuitive (Haviva & Starzyk, 2022 ). The research community learns perhaps more from these null findings than from expected ones. Open science practices such as preregistration ensure that high-quality research contributes to the general body of knowledge regardless of research outcome, reducing publication bias and supporting a more transparent and comprehensive portrait of science (Nosek & Lakens, 2014 ).

Aviezer, H., Ensenberg, N., & Hassin, R. R. (2017). The inherently contextualized nature of facial emotion perception. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17 , 47–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.06.006

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Barrett, L. F., & Westlin, C. (2021). Navigating the science of emotion. In H. Meiselman (Ed.), Emotion measurement. Oxford, England: Elsevier.

Google Scholar  

Bryant, G. A., & Aktipis, C. A. (2014). The animal nature of spontaneous human laughter. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35 (4), 327–335. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.03.003

Article   Google Scholar  

Bryant, G. A., Fessler, D. M. T., Fusaroli, R., Clint, E., Aarøe, L., & Apicella, C. L. (2016). Detecting affiliation in colaughter across 24 Societies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113 (17), 4682–87. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1524993113

Cowen, A. S., & Keltner, D. (2019). What the face displays: Mapping 28 emotions conveyed by naturalistic expression. American Psychologist, 75 (3), 349–364. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000488

Crivelli, C., & Fridlund, A. J. (2019). Inside-out: From basic emotions theory to the behavioral ecology view. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 43 , 161–194. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-019-00294-2

Curran, W., McKeown, G. J., Rychlowska, M., André, E., Wagner, J., & Lingenfelser, F. (2018). Social context disambiguates the interpretation of laughter. Frontiers in Psychology, 8 , 2342. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02342

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Darwin, C. (1872/1965). The expression of emotions in man and animals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6 , 169–200.

Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). The repertoire of nonverbal behavior: Categories, origins, usage, and coding. Semiotica, 1 , 49–98.

Farley, S. D., Carson, D., & Hughes, S. M. (2022). Just seconds of laughter reveals relationship status – Laughter with friends sounds more authentic and less vulnerable than laughter with romantic partners. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior . https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-022-00406-5

Gervais, M., & Wilson, D. S. (2005). The evolution and functions of laughter and humor: A synthetic approach. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 80 (4), 395–430. https://doi.org/10.1086/498281

Haviva, C. & Starzyk, K. B. (2022). Zero to 60 Laughs per Hour: Observed laughter, physical health, personality, and well-being in people aged 67 to 95, an exploratory study. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior .

Keltner, D., Sauter, D., Tracy, J. L., & Cowen, A. (2019a). Emotional expression: Advances in basic emotion theory. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 43 (2), 133–160. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-019-00293-3

Keltner, D., Tracy, J. L., Sauter, D., & Cowen, A. (2019b). Commentary: What basic emotion theory really says for the 21st century study of emotion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 43 (2), 195–201. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-019-00298-y

Mazzocconi, C., & Ginzburg, J. (2022). A longitudinal characterization of typical laughter development in Mother–child interaction from 12 to 36 months: Formal features and reciprocal responsiveness. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior . https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-022-00403-8

Namba, S., Makihara, S., Kabir, R. S., Miyatani, M., & Nakao, T. (2017). Spontaneous facial expressions are different from posed facial expressions: Morphological properties and dynamic sequences. Current Psychology, 36 , 593–605. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-016-9448-9

Nosek, B. A., & Lakens, D. (2014). Registered reports: A method to increase the credibility of published results. Social Psychology, 45 (3), 137–141. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000192

Provine, R. R. (2001).  Laughter: A scientific investigation. Penguin Press.

Provine, R. R. (2004). Laughing, tickling, and the evolution of speech and self. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13 (6), 215–218. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00311.x

Rychlowska, M., McKeown, G. J., Sneddon, I., & Curran, W. (2022). The role of contextual information in classifying spontaneous social laughter. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior.

Smartt, T., Talaifar, S., & Gosling, S. D. (2022). Dostoyevsky’s conjecture: Evaluating personality impressions based on laughter. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior . https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-022-00408-3

Vouloumanos, A., & Bryant, G. A. (2019). Five-month-old infants detect affiliation in colaughter. Scientific Reports, 9 , 4158. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-38954-4

Wood, A., Sievert, S., & Martin, J. (2022). Semantic similarity of social functional smiles and laughter. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior . https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-022-00405-6

Download references

This manuscript was not supported by funding of any kind.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

College of Arts and Sciences, University of Baltimore, 1420 N. Charles Street, Learning Commons 405, Baltimore, MD, 21201, USA

Sally D. Farley

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

Sally Farley was the sole author on this manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sally D. Farley .

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest.

Sally Farley is the associate editor for special issues at the Journal of Nonverbal Behavior.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Farley, S.D. Introduction to the Special Issue of the Scientific Study of Laughter: Where We Have Been, Current Innovations, and Where We Might Go From Here. J Nonverbal Behav 46 , 321–326 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-022-00413-6

Download citation

Accepted : 05 August 2022

Published : 16 August 2022

Issue Date : December 2022

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-022-00413-6

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Representational
  • Constructionist
  • Functionalist
  • Dual pathway
  • Spontaneous
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

two girls laughing very hard

Laughing is good for your mind and your body – here’s what the research shows

research essay on laughter

Professor of Cognitive Psychology, Grinnell College

Disclosure statement

Janet M. Gibson does not work for, consult, own shares in or receive funding from any company or organisation that would benefit from this article, and has disclosed no relevant affiliations beyond their academic appointment.

View all partners

Amusement and pleasant surprises – and the laughter they can trigger – add texture to the fabric of daily life.

Those giggles and guffaws can seem like just silly throwaways. But laughter, in response to funny events, actually takes a lot of work, because it activates many areas of the brain : areas that control motor, emotional, cognitive and social processing.

As I found when writing “ An Introduction to the Psychology of Humor ,” researchers now appreciate laughter’s power to enhance physical and mental well-being.

Laughter’s physical power

People begin laughing in infancy, when it helps develop muscles and upper body strength . Laughter is not just breathing. It relies on complex combinations of facial muscles, often involving movement of the eyes, head and shoulders.

Laughter – doing it or observing it – activates multiple regions of the brain: the motor cortex, which controls muscles; the frontal lobe, which helps you understand context; and the limbic system, which modulates positive emotions. Turning all these circuits on strengthens neural connections and helps a healthy brain coordinate its activity.

By activating the neural pathways of emotions like joy and mirth, laughter can improve your mood and make your physical and emotional response to stress less intense. For example, laughing may help control brain levels of the neurotransmitter serotonin, similar to what antidepressants do. By minimizing your brain’s responses to threats, it limits the release of neurotransmitters and hormones like cortisol that can wear down your cardiovascular, metabolic and immune systems over time. Laughter’s kind of like an antidote to stress , which weakens these systems and increases vulnerability to diseases.

women laughing together at an outdoor meal

Laughter’s cognitive power

A good sense of humor and the laughter that follows depend on an ample measure of social intelligence and working memory resources.

Laughter, like humor, typically sparks from recognizing the incongruities or absurdities of a situation. You need to mentally resolve the surprising behavior or event – otherwise you won’t laugh; you might just be confused instead. Inferring the intentions of others and taking their perspective can enhance the intensity of the laughter and amusement you feel.

To “get” a joke or humorous situation, you need to be able to see the lighter side of things. You must believe that other possibilities besides the literal exist – think about being amused by comic strips with talking animals , like those found in “ The Far Side .”

Laughter’s social power

Many cognitive and social skills work together to help you monitor when and why laughter occurs during conversations. You don’t even need to hear a laugh to be able to laugh. Deaf signers punctuate their signed sentences with laughter , much like emoticons in written text.

Laughter creates bonds and increases intimacy with others. Linguist Don Nilsen points out that chuckles and belly laughs seldom happen when alone , supporting their strong social role. Beginning early in life, infants’ laughter is an external sign of pleasure that helps strengthen bonds with caregivers.

Later, it’s an external sign of sharing an appreciation of the situation. For example, public speakers and comedians try to get a laugh to make audiences feel psychologically closer to them, to create intimacy.

By practicing a little laughter each day, you can enhance social skills that may not come naturally to you. When you laugh in response to humor, you share your feelings with others and learn from risks that your response will be accepted/shared/enjoyed by others and not be rejected/ignored/disliked.

In studies, psychologists have found that men with Type A personality characteristics , including competitiveness and time urgency, tend to laugh more, while women with those traits laugh less. Both sexes laugh more with others than when alone.

white-haired woman laughing on a park bench

Laughter’s mental power

Positive psychology researchers study how people can live meaningful lives and thrive. Laughter produces positive emotions that lead to this kind of flourishing. These feelings – like amusement, happiness, mirth and joy – build resiliency and increase creative thinking . They increase subjective well-being and life satisfaction . Researchers find that these positive emotions experienced with humor and laughter correlate with appreciating the meaning of life and help older adults hold a benign view of difficulties they’ve faced over a lifetime.

Laughter in response to amusement is a healthy coping mechanism. When you laugh, you take yourself or the situation less seriously and may feel empowered to problem-solve . For example, psychologists measured the frequency and intensity of 41 people’s laughter over two weeks, along with their ratings of physical and mental stress. They found that the more laughter experienced , the lower the reported stress. Whether the instances of laughter were strong, medium or weak in intensity didn’t matter.

Maybe you want to grab some of these benefits for yourself – can you force laughter to work for you?

A growing number of therapists advocate using humor and laughter to help clients build trust and improve work environments ; a review of five different studies found that measures of well-being did increase after laughter interventions. Sometimes called homeplay instead of homework, these interventions take the form of daily humor activities – surrounding yourself with funny people, watching a comedy that makes you laugh or writing down three funny things that happened today.

[ Deep knowledge, daily. Sign up for The Conversation’s newsletter .]

You can practice laughing even when alone. Intentionally take a perspective that appreciates the funny side of events. Laughing yoga is a technique of using breathing muscles to achieve the positive physical responses of natural laughing with forced laughter (ha ha hee hee ho ho).

Researchers today certainly aren’t laughing off its value, but a good deal of the research on laughter’s influence on mental and physical health is based on self-report measures. More psychological experimentation around laughter or the contexts in which it occurs will likely support the importance of laughing throughout your day, and maybe even suggest more ways to intentionally harness its benefits.

  • Positive psychology
  • Emotional well being
  • Human brain
  • Social bonds
  • Mental well-being
  • Social bonding
  • Emotional well-being
  • Curiosity question
  • Psychology of humor
  • Positive emotions

research essay on laughter

Audience Development Coordinator (fixed-term maternity cover)

research essay on laughter

Data and Reporting Analyst

research essay on laughter

Lecturer (Hindi-Urdu)

research essay on laughter

Director, Defence and Security

research essay on laughter

Opportunities with the new CIEHF

  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Therapy Center
  • When To See a Therapist
  • Types of Therapy
  • Best Online Therapy
  • Best Couples Therapy
  • Best Family Therapy
  • Managing Stress
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Understanding Emotions
  • Self-Improvement
  • Healthy Relationships
  • Student Resources
  • Personality Types
  • Guided Meditations
  • Verywell Mind Insights
  • 2023 Verywell Mind 25
  • Mental Health in the Classroom
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Review Board
  • Crisis Support

The Health Benefits of Laughter

Elizabeth Scott, PhD is an author, workshop leader, educator, and award-winning blogger on stress management, positive psychology, relationships, and emotional wellbeing.

research essay on laughter

Rachel Goldman, PhD FTOS, is a licensed psychologist, clinical assistant professor, speaker, wellness expert specializing in eating behaviors, stress management, and health behavior change.

research essay on laughter

Research has shown that the health benefits of laughter are far-ranging. Studies so far have shown that laughter can help relieve pain, bring greater happiness, and even increase immunity. Positive psychology names the propensity for laughter and sense of humor as one of the 24 main signature strengths one can possess.  

Unfortunately, however, many people don't get enough laughter in their lives.

In fact, one study suggests that healthy children may laugh as much as 400 times per day, but adults tend to laugh only 15 times per day.

Other studies find us laughing a little more than that, but if you ask me, virtually all of us could use a little more laughter in our lives, considering how beneficial a good laugh can actually be for our stress levels and overall wellness. 

Stress Management Benefits of Laughter

Here are some of the many ways laughter reduces stress.

Laughter reduces the level of stress hormones like cortisol , epinephrine (adrenaline), dopamine , and growth hormone. It also increases the level of health-enhancing hormones, like endorphins.

Laughter increases the number of antibody-producing cells we have working for us and enhances the effectiveness of T cells. All this means a stronger immune system, as well as fewer physical effects of stress.  

Physical Release

Have you ever felt like you have to laugh or you'll cry? Have you experienced that cleansed feeling after a good laugh? Laughter provides a physical and emotional release.

Internal Workout

A good belly laugh exercises the diaphragm, contracts the abs, and even works out the shoulders, leaving muscles more relaxed afterward. It even provides a good workout for the heart.

Distraction

Laughter brings the focus away from anger , guilt, stress , and negative emotions in a more beneficial way than other mere distractions.

Perspective

Studies show that our response to stressful events can be altered by whether we view something as a threat or a challenge . Humor can give us a more lighthearted perspective and help us view events as challenges, thereby making them less threatening and more positive.  

Social Benefits

Laughter connects us with others. Just as with smiling and kindness , most people find that laughter is contagious. So, if you bring more laughter into your life, you can most likely help others around you to laugh more and realize these benefits as well.

By elevating the mood of those around you, you can reduce their stress levels and perhaps improve the quality of social interaction you experience with them, reducing your stress level even more!

How to Use Laughter

Laughter is one of my all-time favorite stress management strategies because it's free, convenient, and beneficial in so many ways. You can get more laughter in your life with the following strategies.

Laugh With Friends

Going to a movie or a comedy club with friends is a great way to get more laughter in your life. The contagious effects of laughter may mean you'll laugh more than you otherwise would have during the show, plus you'll have jokes to reference at later times.

Having friends over for a party or game night is also a great setup for laughter and other good feelings. Making time for this kind of fun is as important as any other habit you keep in your life to support your health, and it's very possibly more enjoyable than most health habits as well.

Find Humor in Your Life

Instead of complaining about life's frustrations, try to laugh about them. If something is so frustrating or depressing it's ridiculous, realize that you could 'look back on it and laugh.' Think of how it will sound like a story you could tell your friends, and then see if you can laugh about it now.

With this attitude, you may also find yourself being more lighthearted and silly, giving yourself and those around you more to laugh about. Approach life in a more mirthful way and you'll find you're less stressed about negative events.

Fake It Until You Make It

Just as studies show the positive effects of smiling occur whether the smile is fake or real , faked laughter also provides the benefits mentioned above. The body can't distinguish between 'fake' laughter that you just start doing on purpose and 'real' laughter that comes from true humor.

The physical benefits are exactly the same, and the former usually leads to the latter anyway. So smile more, and fake laughter; you'll still achieve positive effects, and the fake merriment may lead to real smiles and laughter.

There's no shortage of laughter opportunities from entertainment, both at the theater as well as at home with streaming movies and T.V. comedies.

While wasting your time watching something marginally funny may actually frustrate you, watching truly hilarious movies and shows is an easy way to get laughter into your life whenever you need it. You may want to share your recommendations with friends and you'll have something to reference and laugh about together.

Wellenzohn S, Proyer RT, Ruch W. Who Benefits From Humor-Based Positive Psychology Interventions? The Moderating Effects of Personality Traits and Sense of Humor . Front Psychol. 2018;9:821. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00821

Savage BM, Lujan HL, Thipparthi RR, Dicarlo SE. Humor, laughter, learning, and health! A brief review . Adv Physiol Educ. 2017;41(3):341-347. doi:10.1152/advan.00030.2017

Skinner N, Brewer N. The dynamics of threat and challenge appraisals prior to stressful achievement events . J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;83(3):678-92. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.83.3.678

  • Bennett MP, Lengacher C. Humor, and Laughter May Influence Health: III. Laughter and Health Outcomes. Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine.
  • Bennett MP, Zeller JM, Rosenberg L, McCann J. The Effect of Mirthful Laughter on Stress and Natural Killer Cell Activity. . Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine.
  • Berk LS, Felten DL, Tan SA, Bittman BB, Westengard J. Modulation of Neuroimmune Parameters During the Eustress of Humor-Associated Mirthful Laughter. . Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine.
  • Research is showing healthful effects of laughter. Family Practice News.
  • Skinner N, Brewer N. The Dynamics of Threat and Challenge Appraisals Prior to Stressful Achievement Events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.

By Elizabeth Scott, PhD Elizabeth Scott, PhD is an author, workshop leader, educator, and award-winning blogger on stress management, positive psychology, relationships, and emotional wellbeing.

Thomas R. Verny M.D.

Laughing Your Way to Health and Joy

Fake it until you make it..

Posted January 4, 2022 | Reviewed by Michelle Quirk

  • Because the mind is not limited to the brain but dwells in the whole body, if the body feels good, the mind will feel good.
  • Mental stress causes blood vessels to constrict contributing to atherosclerosis and heart disease.
  • Some health professionals are implementing laughter therapy in their clinical practice, both individually and in groups.

Freesvg Clip art public domain

I am sure the reader is familiar with the concept of psychosomatics — the science of how the mind affects the body, for better or worse. Many people forget that the reverse is also true. What goes on in the body affects the psyche — in other words, the mind. When we are happy, we smile or laugh. When we are sad, we sob, we cry. The interesting thing is that when we pretend to cry or laugh — actors do this all the time — the feelings associated with these bodily changes well up. In these difficult times, conscious laughter may be just the ticket to feeling more optimistic and cheerful.

The Historical Perspective

In India, laughing yoga or yoga laughter has existed for a very long time. Nobody really knows who invented it or when it first appeared. Whatever its origins, laughter yoga owes its modern success to a family physician, Madan Kataria, and his wife, Madhuri Kataria, who modernized its practice by making it accessible to Westerners.

In the United States, William F. Fry, a professor of psychology at Stanford University, California, was the first scientist to suggest in 1964 that laughter was a suitable field of study. The American Association for Therapeutic Humor was founded in 1987, and the formation of the International Society for Humor Studies followed in 1988. Both of these organizations unite several hundred professionals interested in integrating humor into a variety of therapeutic modalities. Together they have published hundreds of articles on the health benefits of laughter, and their members have written many books and spoken at numerous conferences.

Marí Cruz Garcia independently developed her own concept of “conscious laughter” in the early 1980s, created her own laughter school, started to train laughter therapists in 1990, and continues to do so.

You may have seen the 1998 Hollywood movie on Dr. Patch Adams starring Robin Williams. Hunter Patch Adams, while still in medical school, committed his life to bringing fun and laughter to young hospitalized patients. Around the same time, professional clowns from the not-for-profit Big Apple Circus started to do hospital visits. Their message and techniques spread worldwide over the following years.

How to Laugh

The easiest way to laugh is in a group led by a person familiar with laughing techniques. For example, see the work of Steve Wilson, a psychologist from Ohio, who brought the concept of Laughter Clubs to the United States in the late 1990s. He has trained thousands of Certified Laughter Leaders in North America and beyond. His approach incorporates laughter exercises, games, music, movement, and creative arts.

Another person whose work you can access on the Web is Annette Goodheart, a psychologist from Santa Barbara, CA. She devised a set of techniques on how to use laughter to release strong or repressed emotions. Her book Laughter Therapy is a good read.

If joining a group either in your city or virtually does not appeal to you, you can learn to laugh on your own. You do not need to have a sense of humor , know jokes, or even be happy. Laugh for the same reason you exercise or diet — because you know it is good for you.

Why It Works

Current scientific research at Loma Linda University’s School of Allied Health and Medicine has shown that laughter helps the brain regulate the stress hormones cortisol and epinephrine. The researchers also discovered a link between laughter and the production of antibodies and endorphins, the body’s natural painkillers. Even the expectation that something funny is coming suffices to bring about positive effects, according to the lead immunologist, Lee Berk.

Scientists at the Maryland School of Medicine found that laughter causes the tissue that forms the inner lining of blood vessels, the endothelium, to expand, resulting in an increased blood flow, while stress constricts blood vessels and reduces blood flow.

In one study volunteers watched segments of a funny movie, "There's Something About Mary" on one day and on the next watched the opening section of the rather stressful movie "Saving Private Ryan." Each volunteer served as his or her own control. Overall, more than 300 measurements were taken. “The magnitude of change we saw in the endothelium after laughing was consistent and similar to the benefit we might see with aerobic exercise or statin use,” said Michael Miller, lead investigator.

Geneticist Kazu Murakami and Yoshimoto Kogyo Co Ltd, a leading entertainment company, jointly carried out their first experiment to let people with diabetes laugh at a comedy show performed by the firm's top stand-up comedians after listening to a monotonous college lecture. The two-day experiment, as described in his 2006 paper, showed that their blood glucose levels dropped after they laughed compared with after listening to the lecture. His latest experiment with the entertainment firm found 18 genes controlling the immune system activated.

research essay on laughter

When we laugh, extra amounts of serotonin are naturally released in the gastrointestinal tract. The parasympathetic system — rest and digest — is dialed up, the sympathetic nervous system — fight or flight — is dialed down. As a result, the immune system is powered up and you heal faster and are less prone to sickness. The pituitary gland and the hypothalamus release endorphins, dopamine , and oxytocin , neuropeptides related to feelings of happiness and bonding . You feel good when you are connected with yourself and others.

I should point out that sufficient randomized controlled clinical trials have not been conducted validating the therapeutic efficacy of laughter. Therefore, it is not surprising that, in academic circles, this approach is generally held in disdain as nonscientific and off the wall and not a replacement for antidepressant medications and psychotherapy . In terms of consumers, the problem is that laughter is free, and we don’t value what’s free.

Laughter, including simulated laughter, has positive quantifiable physiological and psychological effects. It can be used with preventive intent or as a complementary or alternative therapeutic option in the treatment of emotional as well as physical disorders. Whether used in a clinical setting or in a laughing yoga group or in the privacy of one’s home, laughter’s benefits are many and can contribute greatly to living life optimally.

Cousins, N. (1976). Anatomy of an illness (as perceived by the patient). New England Journal of Medicine, 295(26), 1458–1463.

Hayashi, T., Urayama, O., Kawai, K., Hayashi, K., Iwanaga, S., Ohta, M., ... & Murakami, K. (2006). Laughter regulates gene expression in patients with type 2 diabetes. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics , 75(1), 62–65.

Miller, M., & Fry, W. F. (2009). The effect of mirthful laughter on the human cardiovascular system. Medical Hypotheses , 73(5), 636–639.

Mora-Ripoll, R. (2013). Laughter techniques for therapeutic use in medicine. OA Alternative Medicine, 1(3), 25.

Tremayne, P., & Sharma, K. (2019). Implementing laughter therapy to enhance the well-being of patients and nurses. Nursing Standard, 34(3).

350+ Scientific Research Papers on Laughter https://www.laughteronlineuniversity.com/350-scientific-research-papers…

Thomas R. Verny M.D.

Thomas R. Verny, M.D. , the author of eight books, including The Embodied Mind , has taught at Harvard University, University of Toronto, York University, and St. Mary’s University of Minnesota. His podcast, Pushing Boundaries , may be viewed on Youtube or listened to on Spotify and many other platforms.

  • Find a Therapist
  • Find a Treatment Center
  • Find a Psychiatrist
  • Find a Support Group
  • Find Teletherapy
  • United States
  • Brooklyn, NY
  • Chicago, IL
  • Houston, TX
  • Los Angeles, CA
  • New York, NY
  • Portland, OR
  • San Diego, CA
  • San Francisco, CA
  • Seattle, WA
  • Washington, DC
  • Asperger's
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Chronic Pain
  • Eating Disorders
  • Passive Aggression
  • Personality
  • Goal Setting
  • Positive Psychology
  • Stopping Smoking
  • Low Sexual Desire
  • Relationships
  • Child Development
  • Therapy Center NEW
  • Diagnosis Dictionary
  • Types of Therapy

March 2024 magazine cover

Understanding what emotional intelligence looks like and the steps needed to improve it could light a path to a more emotionally adept world.

  • Coronavirus Disease 2019
  • Affective Forecasting
  • Neuroscience

Managing Conflict with Humor

Finding joy during difficult times, improving emotional intelligence (eq).

  • Imposter Syndrome: Causes, Types, and Coping Tips

Personality Types, Traits, and How it Affects Mental Health

Gratitude: the benefits and how to practice it, prayers for anxiety, healing, and coping with life’s challenges, social media and mental health.

  • Online Therapy: Is it Right for You?
  • Mental Health
  • Health & Wellness
  • Children & Family
  • Relationships

Are you or someone you know in crisis?

  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Eating Disorders
  • Grief & Loss
  • Personality Disorders
  • PTSD & Trauma
  • Schizophrenia
  • Therapy & Medication
  • Exercise & Fitness
  • Healthy Eating
  • Well-being & Happiness
  • Weight Loss
  • Work & Career
  • Illness & Disability
  • Heart Health
  • Childhood Issues
  • Learning Disabilities
  • Family Caregiving
  • Teen Issues
  • Communication
  • Emotional Intelligence
  • Love & Friendship
  • Domestic Abuse
  • Healthy Aging
  • Aging Issues
  • Alzheimer’s Disease & Dementia
  • Senior Housing
  • End of Life
  • Meet Our Team

The benefits of laughter

What are the physical, mental, and social benefits of laughter, laughter helps you stay mentally healthy, laughter brings people together and strengthens relationships, how to bring more laughter into your life, tips for developing your sense of humor, using humor to overcome challenges and enhance your life, laughter is the best medicine.

It’s fun to share a good laugh, but did you know it can actually improve your health? Learn how to harness the powerful benefits of laughter and humor.

research essay on laughter

It’s true: laughter is strong medicine. It draws people together in ways that trigger healthy physical and emotional changes in the body. Laughter strengthens your immune system, boosts mood, diminishes pain, and protects you from the damaging effects of stress. Nothing works faster or more dependably to bring your mind and body back into balance than a good laugh. Humor lightens your burdens, inspires hope, connects you to others, and keeps you grounded, focused, and alert. It also helps you release anger and forgive sooner.

With so much power to heal and renew, the ability to laugh easily and frequently is a tremendous resource for surmounting problems, enhancing your relationships, and supporting both physical and emotional health. Best of all, this priceless medicine is fun, free, and easy to use.

As children, we used to laugh hundreds of times a day, but as adults, life tends to be more serious and laughter more infrequent. But by seeking out more opportunities for humor and laughter, you can improve your emotional health, strengthen your relationships, find greater happiness—and even add years to your life.

Laughter is good for your health

Laughter relaxes the whole body. A good, hearty laugh relieves physical tension and stress, leaving your muscles relaxed for up to 45 minutes after.

Laughter boosts the immune system. Laughter decreases stress hormones and increases immune cells and infection-fighting antibodies, thus improving your resistance to disease.

Laughter triggers the release of endorphins, the body’s natural feel-good chemicals. Endorphins promote an overall sense of well-being and can even temporarily relieve pain.

Laughter protects the heart. Laughter improves the function of blood vessels and increases blood flow, which can help protect you against a heart attack and other cardiovascular problems.

Laughter burns calories. Okay, so it’s no replacement for going to the gym, but one study found that laughing for 10 to 15 minutes a day can burn approximately 40 calories—which could be enough to lose three or four pounds over the course of a year.

Laughter lightens anger’s heavy load . Nothing diffuses anger and conflict faster than a shared laugh. Looking at the funny side can put problems into perspective and enable you to move on from confrontations without holding onto bitterness or resentment.

Laughter may even help you to live longer. A study in Norway found that people with a strong sense of humor outlived those who don’t laugh as much. The difference was particularly notable for those battling cancer.

Physical health benefits

  • Boosts immunity
  • Lowers stress hormones
  • Decreases pain
  • Relaxes your muscles
  • Prevents heart disease

Mental health benefits

  • Adds joy and zest to life
  • Eases anxiety and tension
  • Relieves stress
  • Improves mood
  • Strengthens resilience

Social benefits

  • Strengthens relationships
  • Attracts others to us
  • Enhances teamwork
  • Helps defuse conflict
  • Promotes group bonding

Laughter makes you feel good. And this positive feeling remains with you even after the laughter subsides. Humor helps you keep a positive, optimistic outlook through difficult situations, disappointments, and loss.

[Read: Cultivating Happiness]

More than just a respite from sadness and pain, laughter gives you the courage and strength to find new sources of meaning and hope. Even in the most difficult of times, a laugh–or even simply a smile–can go a long way toward making you feel better. And laughter really is contagious—just hearing laughter primes your brain and readies you to smile and join in the fun.

The link between laughter and mental health

Laughter stops distressing emotions. You can’t feel anxious, angry, or sad when you’re laughing.

Laughter helps you relax and recharge. It reduces stress and increases energy, enabling you to stay focused and accomplish more.

Laughter shifts perspective , allowing you to see situations in a more realistic, less threatening light. A humorous perspective creates psychological distance, which can help you avoid feeling overwhelmed and diffuse conflict.

Laughter draws you closer to others, which can have a profound effect on all aspects of your mental and emotional health.

Speak to a Licensed Therapist

BetterHelp is an online therapy service that matches you to licensed, accredited therapists who can help with depression, anxiety, relationships, and more. Take the assessment and get matched with a therapist in as little as 48 hours.

There’s a good reason why TV sitcoms use laugh tracks: laughter is contagious. You’re many times more likely to laugh around other people than when you’re alone. And the more laughter you bring into your own life, the happier you and those around you will feel.

Sharing humor is half the fun—in fact, most laughter doesn’t come from hearing jokes, but rather simply from spending time with friends and family. And it’s this social aspect that plays such an important role in the health benefits of laughter. You can’t enjoy a laugh with other people unless you take the time to really engage with them. When you care about someone enough to switch off your phone and really connect face to face, you’re engaging in a process that rebalances the nervous system and puts the brakes on defensive stress responses like “fight or flight.” And if you share a laugh as well, you’ll both feel happier, more positive, and more relaxed—even if you’re unable to alter a stressful situation.

How laughing together can strengthen relationships

Shared laughter is one of the most effective tools for keeping relationships fresh and exciting. All emotional sharing builds strong and lasting relationship bonds, but sharing laughter also adds joy, vitality, and resilience. And humor is a powerful and effective way to heal resentments, disagreements, and hurts. Laughter unites people during difficult times.

Humor and playful communication strengthen our relationships by triggering positive feelings and fostering emotional connection. When we laugh with one another, a positive bond is created. This bond acts as a strong buffer against stress, disagreements, and disappointment. Humor and laughter in relationships allows you to:

Be more spontaneous. Humor gets you out of your head and away from your troubles.

Let go of defensiveness. Laughter helps you forget resentments, judgments, criticisms, and doubts.

Release inhibitions. Your fear of holding back is pushed aside.

Express your true feelings. Deeply felt emotions are allowed to rise to the surface.

Use humor to resolve disagreements and tension in your relationship

Laughter is an especially powerful tool for managing conflict and reducing tension when emotions are running high. Whether with romantic partners, friends and family, or co-workers, you can learn to use humor to smooth over disagreements , lower everyone’s stress level, and communicate in a way that builds up your relationships rather than breaking them down.

Laughter is your birthright, a natural part of life that is innate and inborn. Infants begin smiling during the first weeks of life and laugh out loud within months of being born. Even if you did not grow up in a household where laughter was a common sound, you can learn to laugh at any stage of life.

Begin by setting aside special times to seek out humor and laughter, as you might with exercising, and build from there. Eventually, you’ll want to incorporate humor and laughter into the fabric of your life, finding it naturally in everything.

Here are some ways to start:

Smile. Smiling is the beginning of laughter, and like laughter, it’s contagious. When you look at someone or see something even mildly pleasing, practice smiling. Instead of looking down at your phone, look up and smile at people you pass in the street, the person serving you a morning coffee, or the co-workers you share an elevator with. Notice the effect on others.

Count your blessings. Literally make a list. The simple act of considering the positive aspects of your life will distance you from negative thoughts that block humor and laughter. When you’re in a state of sadness, you have further to travel to reach humor and laughter.

[Listen: Gratitude in Difficult Times]

When you hear laughter, move toward it. Sometimes humor and laughter are private, a shared joke among a small group, but usually not. More often, people are very happy to share something funny because it gives them an opportunity to laugh again and feed off the humor you find in it. When you hear laughter, seek it out and ask, “What’s funny?”

Spend time with fun, playful people. These are people who laugh easily–both at themselves and at life’s absurdities–and who routinely find the humor in everyday events. Their playful point of view and laughter are contagious. Even if you don’t consider yourself a lighthearted, humorous person, you can still seek out people who like to laugh and make others laugh. Every comedian appreciates an audience.

Bring humor into conversations. Ask people, “What’s the funniest thing that happened to you today? This week? In your life?”

Simulated laughter

So, what if you really can’t “find the funny?” Believe it or not, it’s possible to laugh without experiencing a funny event—and simulated laughter can be just as beneficial as the real thing. It can even make exercise more fun and productive. A Georgia State University study found that incorporating bouts of simulated laughter into an exercise program helped improve older adults’ mental health as well as their aerobic endurance. Plus, hearing others laugh, even for no apparent reason, can often trigger genuine laughter.

To add simulated laughter into your own life, search for laugh yoga or laugh therapy groups. Or you can start simply by laughing at other people’s jokes, even if you don’t find them funny. Both you and the other person will feel good, it will draw you closer together, and who knows, it may even lead to some spontaneous laughter.

Creating opportunities to laugh

  • Watch a funny movie, TV show, or YouTube video.
  • Invite friends or co-workers out to a comedy club.
  • Read the funny pages.
  • Seek out funny people.
  • Share a good joke or a funny story.
  • Check out your bookstore’s humor section.
  • Host game night with friends.
  • Play with a pet.
  • Go to a “laughter yoga” class.
  • Goof around with children.
  • Do something silly.
  • Make time for fun activities (e.g. bowling, miniature golfing, karaoke).

An essential ingredient for developing your sense of humor is to learn not to take yourself too seriously and laugh at your own mistakes and foibles. As much as we’d like to believe otherwise, we all do foolish things from time to time. Instead of feeling embarrassed or defensive, embrace your imperfections. While some events in life are clearly sad and not opportunities for laughter, most don’t carry an overwhelming sense of either sadness or delight. They fall into the gray zone of ordinary life—giving you the choice to laugh or not. So, choose to laugh whenever you can.

How to develop your sense of humor

Laugh at yourself. Share your embarrassing moments. The best way to take yourself less seriously is to talk about times when you took yourself too seriously.

Attempt to laugh at situations rather than bemoan them. Look for the humor in a bad situation, and uncover the irony and absurdity of life. When something negative happens, try to make it a humorous anecdote that will make others laugh.

Surround yourself with reminders to lighten up. Keep a toy on your desk or in your car. Put up a funny poster in your office. Choose a computer screensaver that makes you laugh. Frame photos of you and your family or friends having fun.

Remember funny things that happen. If something amusing happens or you hear a joke or funny story you really like, write it down or tell it to someone to help you remember it.

Don’t dwell on the negative. Try to avoid negative people and don’t dwell on news stories, entertainment, or conversations that make you sad or unhappy. Many things in life are beyond your control—particularly the behavior of other people. While you might view carrying the weight of the world on your shoulders as admirable, in the long run it’s unrealistic and unhealthy.

Find your inner child. Pay attention to children and try to emulate them—after all, they are the experts on playing, taking life lightly, and laughing at ordinary things.

Deal with stress. Stress can be a major impediment to humor and laughter, so it’s important to keep your stress levels in check. One great technique to relieve stress in the moment is to draw upon a favorite memory that always makes you smile—something your kids did, for example, or something funny a friend told you.

Don’t go a day without laughing. Think of it like exercise or breakfast and make a conscious effort to find something each day that makes you laugh. Set aside 10 to 15 minutes and do something that amuses you. The more you get used to laughing each day, the less effort you’ll have to make.

The ability to laugh, play, and have fun not only makes life more enjoyable but also helps you solve problems, connect with others, and think more creatively. People who incorporate humor and play into their daily lives find that it renews them and all of their relationships.

Life brings challenges that can either get the best of you or become playthings for your imagination. When you “become the problem” and take yourself too seriously, it can be hard to think outside the box and find new solutions. But when you play with the problem, you can often transform it into an opportunity for creative learning.

[Read: Managing Conflict with Humor]

Playing with problems seems to come naturally to children. When they are confused or afraid, they make their problems into a game, giving them a sense of control and an opportunity to experiment with new solutions. Interacting with others in playful ways helps you retain this creative ability.

Here are two examples of people who took everyday problems and turned them around through laughter and play:

  • Roy , a semi-retired businessman, was excited to finally have time to devote to golf, his favorite sport. But the more he played, the less he enjoyed himself. Although his game had improved dramatically, he got angry with himself over every mistake. Roy wisely realized that his golfing buddies affected his attitude, so he stopped playing with people who took the game too seriously. When he played with friends who focused more on having fun than on their scores, he was less critical of himself. Now golfing was as enjoyable as Roy had envisioned. He scored better without working harder. And the brighter outlook he was gaining from his companions and the game spread to other parts of his life.
  • Jane worked at home designing greeting cards, a job she used to love but now felt had become routine. Two little girls who loved to draw and paint lived next door. Eventually, Jane invited the girls over to play with all of her art supplies. At first, she just watched, but in time she joined in. Laughing, coloring, and playing pretend with the little girls transformed Jane’s life. Not only did it end her loneliness and boredom, but it sparked her imagination and helped her artwork flourish. Best of all, it rekindled the playfulness in Jane’s relationship with her husband.

As laughter, humor, and play become integrated into your life, your creativity will flourish and new opportunities for laughing with friends, coworkers, acquaintances, and loved ones will occur to you daily. Laughter takes you to a higher place where you can view the world from a more relaxed, positive, and joyful perspective.

More Information

  • Buchowski, M. S., Majchrzak, K. M., Blomquist, K., Chen, K. Y., Byrne, D. W., & Bachorowski, J.-A. (2007). Energy expenditure of genuine laughter. International Journal of Obesity , 31(1), 131–137. Link
  • Laugh and be thankful—It’s good for the heart—Harvard Health . (n.d.). Retrieved May 25, 2022, from Link
  • Manninen, S., Tuominen, L., Dunbar, R. I., Karjalainen, T., Hirvonen, J., Arponen, E., Hari, R., Jääskeläinen, I. P., Sams, M., & Nummenmaa, L. (2017). Social Laughter Triggers Endogenous Opioid Release in Humans. The Journal of Neuroscience , 37(25), 6125–6131. Link
  • Miller, M., & Fry, W. F. (2009). The effect of mirthful laughter on the human cardiovascular system. Medical Hypotheses , 73(5), 636–639. /p> Link
  • Romundstad, S., Svebak, S., Holen, A., & Holmen, J. (2016). A 15-Year Follow-Up Study of Sense of Humor and Causes of Mortality: The Nord-Trøndelag Health Study. Psychosomatic Medicine , 78(3), 345–353. Link
  • Speer, M. E., & Delgado, M. R. (2017). Reminiscing about positive memories buffers acute stress responses. Nature Human Behaviour , 1(5), 0093. Link
  • Yim, J. (2016). Therapeutic Benefits of Laughter in Mental Health: A Theoretical Review. The Tohoku Journal of Experimental Medicine , 239(3), 243–249. Link

More in Well-being & Happiness

Using laughter and play to resolve disagreements

research essay on laughter

Daily practices to lift your spirits

research essay on laughter

Boost your emotional intelligence to help you be happy and successful

research essay on laughter

Imposter Syndrome

What to do when you feel like a fraud at work, school, or in relationships

research essay on laughter

Your personality impacts your health, mood, and relationships. Here’s what you need to know.

research essay on laughter

Reaping the mood-boosting effects

research essay on laughter

Find strength, healing, and support

research essay on laughter

Changing your habits to avoid anxiety, depression, isolation, and FOMO

research essay on laughter

Professional therapy, done online

BetterHelp makes starting therapy easy. Take the assessment and get matched with a professional, licensed therapist.

Help us help others

Millions of readers rely on HelpGuide.org for free, evidence-based resources to understand and navigate mental health challenges. Please donate today to help us save, support, and change lives.

Laughter Online University

350+ Scientific Research Papers on Laughter

The following list was compiled by Don L. F. Nilsen, English Department, Arizona State University Tempe, AZ (possibly using http://www.BioMedSearch.com ). You will find more (and more recent) research articles on laughter categorized by area(s) of benefits in the LOU laughter research center .

  • Adelswärd, Viveka, and Britt-Marie Öberg. “The Function of Laughter and Joking in Negotiating Activities.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research 11.4 (1998): 411-430.
  • Arner, T. D. “No Joke: Transcendent Laughter in The Teseida and The Miller’s Tale.” Studies in Philology 102.2 (2005): 143-158.
  • Askenasy, J. J. M. “The Functions and Dysfunctions of Laughter.” Journal of General Psychology 14.4 (1987): 317-34.
  • Bainy, Moses. Why Do We Laugh and Cry? West Ryde, Australia: Sunlight Publications, 1993.
  • Basil Hall, Laughter as a displacement activity: the implications for humor theory
  • Baudelaire, Charles. The Essence of Laughter. New York, NY: Meridian: 1956.
  • Bell, N. D. “Laughter in Interaction.” Discourse Studies 7.1 (2005): 137-138.
  • Bellert, J. “Humor: A Therapeutic Approach in Oncology Nursing.” Cancer Nursing 12.2 (1989): 65-70.
  • Berger, Arthur Asa. “After the Laughter: A Concluding Note.” Blind Men and Elephants: Perspectives on Humor. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Press, 1995, 159-170.
  • Berger, Arthur Asa. “The Functions of Laughter: Sociological Aspects of Humor.” Blind Men and Elephants: Perspectives on Humor. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Press, 1995, 91-104.
  • Berger, Arthur Asa. “The Politics of Laughter: A Cultural Theory of Humor Preferences.” Blind Men and Elephants: Perspectives on Humor. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Press, 1995, 105-120.
  • Berger, Arthur Asa. “The Problem of Laughter: Philosophical Approaches to Humor.” Blind Men and Elephants: Perspectives on Humor. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Press, 1995, 37-50.
  • Berger, Arthur Asa. Redeeming Laughter. New York, NY: Walter de Gruyter, 1997.
  • Berger, Arthur Asa. “The Rhetoric of Laughter: The Techniques Used in Humor.” Blind Men and Elephants: Perspectives on Humor. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Press, 1995, 51-64.
  • Berger, Arthur Asa. “Seeing Laughter: Visual Aspects of Humor.” Blind Men and Elephants: Perspectives on Humor. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Press, 1995, 139-158.
  • Berger, Arthur Asa. “The Structure of Laughter: Semiotics and Humor.” Blind Men and Elephants: Perspectives on Humor. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Press, 1995, 65-78.
  • Berger, Arthur Asa, and A. Wildavsky. “Who Laughs at What?” Society 31.6 (1994): 82-86.
  • Berger, Peter L. Redeeming Laughter: The Comic Dimension of Human Experience. Hawthorne, NY: Walter de Gruyter, 1997.
  • Berger, Phil. The Last Laugh. NY: Limelight, 1985.
  • Bergler, E. Laughter and the Sense of Humor. NY: Intercontinental Medical Book Corp, 1956.
  • Bergson, Henri Louis. Laughter: An Essay on the Meaning of the Comic. New York, NY: MacMillan, 1924.
  • Bergson, Henri Louis. “Laughter.” Comedy. Ed. Wylie Sypher. Garden City, NY: Doubleday, 1956, 59-190.
  • Berk, Lee, and S. A. Tan. “Eustress of Mirthful Laughter Modulates the Immune System Lymphokine Interferon-Gama.” Annals of Behavioral Medicine Supplement, Proceedings of the Society of Behavioral Medicine’s Sixteenth Annual Scientific Sessions 17 (1995): C064.
  • Berk, Lee, S. A. Tan, and William Fry. “Eustress of Humor Associated Laughter Modulates Specific Immune System Components.” Annals of Behavioral Medicine Supplement, Proceedings of the Society of Behavioral Medicine’s Fourteenth Annual Scientific Sessions 15 S111.
  • Berk, Lee., S. A. Tan, William F. Fry, B. J. Napier, J. W. Lee, R. W. Hubbard, J. E. Lewis, and W. C. Eby. “Neuroendrocrine and Stress Hormone Changes During Mirthful Laughter.” American Journal of the Medical Sciences. 298.6 (1989): 390-96.
  • Berk, Lee, S. Tan, B. Napier, and W. Evy. “Eustress of Mirthful Laughter Modifies Natural Killer Cell Activity. Clinical Research 37 (1989): 115A.
  • Berk, Lee, S. A. Tan, S. Nehlsen-Cannarella, B. J. Napier, J. E. Lewis, J. E. Lee, and W. C. Eby. “Humor Associated with Laughter Decreases Cortisol and Increases Spontaneous Lymphocyte Blastogenesis.” Clinical Research 36 (1988): 435A.
  • Berlyne, D. E. “Laughter, Humor, and Play.” Handbook of Social Psychology: Volume 3. Eds. G. Lindzey, and E. Aronson. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley, 1969.
  • Bizi, S., G. Keinan, and B. Beit-Hallahi. “Humor and Coping with Stress: A Test Under Real-Life conditions.” Personality and Individual Differences 9 (1988): 951-956.
  • Black, D. W. “Laughter.” Journal of the American Medical Association 252.21 (1984): 2995-98.
  • Black, D. W. “Pathological Laughter.” Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases 170 (1982): 67-71.
  • Blumenfeld, E., and L. Alpern. The Smile Connection: How to Use Humor in Dealilng with People. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, 1986.
  • Bonaiuto, Marino, Elio Castellana, and Antonio Pierro. “Arguing and Laughing: The Use of Humor to Negotiate in Group Discussions.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research 16.2 (2003): 183-224.
  • Bornstein, M. H., and M. E. Arterberry. “Recognition, Discrimination and Categorization ofSmiling by 5-Month-Old Infants.” Developmental Science 6.5 (2003): 585-599.
  • Boston, Richard. An Anatomy of Laughter. London, England, Collins, 1974.
  • Bouissac, Paul. “A Laughable Theory of Laughter.” High Quality 22 (1992): 8-11.
  • Boyd, B. “Laughter and Literature: A Play Theory of Humor.” Philosophy and Literature 28.1 (2004): 1-22.
  • Braga, S. S., R. Manni, and R. F. Pedretti. “Laughter-Induced Syncope.” Lancet (July 30-August 6, 2005): 366-426.
  • Brody, R. “Anatomy of a Laugh.” American Health. (Dec, 1983): 43-47.
  • Brottman, Mikita. “Risus Sardonicus: Neurotic and Pathological Laughter.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research. 15.4 (2002): 401-418.
  • Brown, G. E., D. Brown, and J. Ramos. “Effects of a Laughing Versus a Non-Laughing Model on Humor: Responses in College Students.” Psychological Relports 48.1 (1981): 35-40.
  • Brown, G. E., K. J. Wheeler, and M. Cash. “The Effects of a Laughing vs. a Non-laughing Model on Humor Responses in Preschool Children.” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 29 (1980): 334-39.
  • Buckley, Francis H. The Morality of Laughter. Ann Arobr, MI: University of Michigan Press, 2003.
  • Bushnell, D. D., and T. J. Scheff. “The Cathartic Effects of Laughter on Audiences.” The Study of Humor. Eds. Harvey Mindess and Joy Turek. Los Angeles, CA: Antioch Univ, 1979, 62ff.
  • Caron, James E. “From Ethology to Aesthetics: Evolution as a Theoretical Paradigm for Research on Laughter, Humor, and Other Comic Phenomena.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research 15.3 (2002): 245-282.
  • Carroll, Noel. “Words, Images, and Laughter.” Persistence of Vision 14 (1997): 42-52.
  • Casadonte, Donald. “A Note on the Neuro-Mathematics of Laughter.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research 16.2 (2003): 133-156.
  • Chapman, Antony J. “Humor and Laughter in Social Interaction and Some Implications. Handbook of Humor Research, Volume I. Eds. P. E. McGhee, and J. H. Goldstein. New York, NY: Springer, 1983, 135-157.
  • Chapman, Antony J. “Social Facilitation of Laughter in Children.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 9 (1973): 528-41.
  • Chapman, Antony J., and W. Chapman. “Responsiveness to Humor: Its Dependency upon a Companion’s Humorous Smiling and Laughter.” The Journal of Psychology 88 (1974): 245-52.
  • Chapman, Antony J., and Hugh C. Foot, eds. Humor and Laughter: Theory, Research, and Applications. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction, 1996.
  • Chapman, Antony J., and D. S. Wright. “Social Enhancement of Laughter: An Experimental Analysis of Some Companion Variables.” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 21 (1976): 201-218.
  • Charland, M. “Normes and Laughter in Rhetorical Culture.” Quarterly Journal of Speech 80.3 (1994): 339-342.
  • Chase, Jefferson S. Inciting Laughter: The Development of “Jewish Humor” in 19th Century German Culture. New York, NY: Walter de Gruyter, 1999.
  • Claassens, L. J. M. “Laughter and Tears: Carnivalistic Overtones in the Stories of Sarah and Hagar.” Perspectives in Religious Studies 32.3 (2005): 295-308.
  • Cleveland, Les. Dark Laughter: War in Song and Popular Culture. Westport, CT: Greenwood, 1994.
  • Cogan, R., D. Cogan, W. Waltz, and M. McCue. “Effects of Laughter and Relaxation on Discomfort Thresholds.” Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 10.2 (1987): 139-144.
  • Coser, R. L. “Laughter among Colleagues.” Psychiatry 23 (1960): 81-95.
  • Coser, R. L. “Some Social Functions of Laughter: A Study of Humor in a Hospital Setting.” Human Relations 12.2 (1959): 171-182.
  • Cousins, Norman. “The Laughter Prescription.” The Saturday Evening Post Oct, 1990: 34.
  • Cousins, Norman. “Proving the Power of Laughter.” Psychology Today 23 (1989: 22-25.
  • Cox, Samuel S. Why We Laugh. New York: Benjamin Blom, 1969.
  • Dardick, G. “Learning to Laugh on the Job Principal 69.5 (1990): 32, 34.
  • Darwin, Charles. “Joy, High Spirits, Love, Tender Feelings, Devotion.” The Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals. New York, NY: D. Appleton, 1924, 196-219.
  • Davidhizar, Ruth, and Margaret Bowen. “The Dynamics of Laughter.” Archives of Psychiatric Nursing. 6.2 (1992): 132-137.
  • Davidson, Cathy N. “Laughter without Comedy in For Whom the BellTolls.” Hemingway Notes 3.2 (1973): 609
  • Davis, Jessica Milner. “Taking Humour and Laughter Seriously.” Australian Journal of Comedy. 2.1 (1996): 77-88.
  • de Sousa, Ronald. “When Is It Wrong to Laugh?” The Philosophy of Laughter and Humor. Ed. John Morreall. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press, 1987, 226-249.
  • Debenham, Warren. Laughter on Record: A Comedy Discography. Metuchen, NJ: The Scarecrow Press, 1988.
  • Delahaye, S. “Death by Laughter in Sade and Maupassant.” French Studies Bulletin 96 (2005): 16-17.
  • Derks, Peter, Lynn S. Gillikin, Debbie S. Bartolome-Rull, and Edward H. Bogart. “Laughter and Electroencephalographic Activity.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research 10.3 (1997): 285-300.
  • Devereux, Paul G., and Gerald P. Ginsburg. “Sociality Effects on the Production of Laughter.” Journal of General Psychology “Special Issue on Humor and Laughter” Eds. Mahony, Diana L. and Louis G. Lippman.128.2 (2001): 227-240.
  • Dickie, S. “Joseph Andrews and the Great Laughter Debate.” Studies in Eighteenth Century Culture 34 (2005): 271-332.
  • Dillon, K., B. Minchoff, and K. Baker. “Positive Emotional States and Enhancement of the Immune System.” International Journal of Psychiatric Medicine 15 (1985): 13-18.
  • Ding, G. F., and A. T. Jersild. “A Study of the Laughing and Smiling of Preschool Children.” Journal of Genetic Psychology 40 (1932): 452-472.
  • Dixon, N. “Humor: A Cognitive Alternative to Stress?” Stress and Anxiety. Eds. I. Sarason and C. Spielberger. 7 (1980): 281-289.
  • Dobbin, J. Individual Differences in th Appraisal of Stress and the Immunological Consequences: Psychological Moderation of Lymphocyte Activation and Cytokine Production. Ontario, Canada: University of Western Ontario, 1990.
  • Donoghu, E. E., M. W. McCarrey, and R. Clement. “Humor Appreciation as a Function of Canned Laughter, A Mirthful Companion, and Field Dependence–Facilation and Inhibitory Effects.” Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science 15.2 (1983): 150-162.
  • Doskoch, P. “Happily ever Laughter.” Psychology Today July/August, 1996): 33-35.
  • Dossey, L. “Now You Are Fit to Live: Humor and Health.” Alternative Therapies 2.5 (1996): 8-13, 98-100.
  • Dudden, Arthur P. The Assault of Laughter. New York, NY: A. S. Barnes, 1962.
  • Dvorakova, Alena. “Laughing at Nothing: Humor as a Response to Nihilism.” British Journal of Aesthetics 45.1 (2005): 106-108.
  • Eastman, Max. Enjoyment of Laughter. NY: Simon and Schuster, 1936.
  • Eckardt, A. Roy. Sitting in the Earth and Laughing: A Handbook of Humor. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction, 1992.
  • Elliot-Binns, C. P. “Laughter and Medicine.” Journal of the Royal Coll Gen Pract 35.8 (1985): 364-65.
  • Elsley, Judy. “Laughter as Feminine Power in The Color Purple and A Question of Silence.” New Perspectives on Women and Comedy. Ed. Regina Barreca. Philadelphia, PA: Gordon and Breach, 1992, 193-200.
  • Erdman, L. “Laughter Therapy for Patients with Cancer.” Oncology Nursing Forum 18.8 (1991): 1359-1363.
  • Falk, Dana R., and Clara E. Hill. “Counselor Interventions Preceding Client Laughter in Brief Therapy.” Journal of Counseling Psychology 39.1 (1992): 39-45.
  • Farley-Hills, David. The Benovelence of Laughter: Comic Poetry of the Commonwealth and Restoration. Totowa, NJ: Rowman and Littlefield, 1974.
  • Fendt, G. “Apartheid among the Dead; Or, on Christian Laughter in Ann Petry’s `The Bones of Louella Brown.'” Contributions in Afroamerican and African Studies 209 (2004): 111-118.
  • Finnigan, Joan. Laughing All the Way Home: Indigenous Humour of the Ottawa Valley. Toronto, Canada: Deneau, 1984.
  • Flewelling, Ralph Tyler. “The Animal Capable of Laughter.” Personalist 25 (1944): 341-353.
  • Flugel, J. C. “Humor and Laughter.” Handbook of Social Psychology. Ed. Lindzey. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley, 1954, 709-34.
  • Fogarasi, A., J. Janszky, Z. Siegler, and I. Tuxhorn. “Ictal Smile Lateralizes to the Right Hemisphere in Childhood Epilepsy.” Epilepsia 46.3 (2005): 449-451.
  • Foot, Hugh. “Humor and Laughter.” A Handbook of Communication Skills. Ed. O. D. W. Hargie. London, England: Croom Helm, 1986, 355-382.
  • Fox, Kathleen. “Laugh it Off: The Effect of Humor on the Well-Being of the Older Adult.” Journal of Gerontological Nursing 16.12 (December, 1990): 11-16.
  • Francis, L. E. “Laughter: The Best Mediation–Humor as Emotion Management Interaction.” Symbolic Interaction 17.2 (1994): 147-163.
  • Frank, Mark G., and Paul Ekman. “Not all Smiles are Created Equal: The Differences between Enjoyment and Nonenjoyment Smiles.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research 6.1 (1993): 6-26.
  • Fridlund, Alan J. “Sociality of Solitary Smiling: Potentiation by an Implicit Audience.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 60 (1991): 229-240.
  • Fridlund, Alan J., and Jennifer M. Loftis. “Relations between Tickling and Humorous Laughter: Preliminary Support for the Darwin-Hecker Hypothesis.” Biological Psychology 30.2 (1990): 141-150.
  • Fry, William F. “Laughter: Is It the Best Medicine?” Stanford MD 10.1 (1971): 16-20.
  • Fry, William F. “The Respiritory Components of Mirthful Laughter.” Journal of Biological Psychology 19.2 (1977): 39-50.
  • Fry, William F., and William M. Savin. “Mirthful Laughter and Blood Pressure.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research 1.1 (1988): 49-62.
  • Gallo, Nick. “Lighten Up: Laugh Your Way to Good Health.” Better Homes and Gardens. August, 1989: 31-32.
  • Gavioli, Lauri. “Turn-Initial Versus Turn-Final Laughter: Two Techniques for Initiating Remedy in English/Italian Bookshop Service Encounters.” Discourse Processes 19 (1995): 369-384.
  • Gazella, Katie. “Humor at the University of Michigan: A New Study Investigates What Makes Us Laugh, and Why.” LSA: College of Literature, Science, and the Arts Magazine (Fall, 2005): 41-42.
  • Gelbart, Larry. Laughing Matters. New York, NY: Random House, 1998.
  • Gelkopf, Marc, Shulamith Kreitler, and Mircea Sigal. “Laughter in a Psychiatric Ward: Somatic, Emotional, Social, and Clinical Influences on Schizophrenic Patients.” Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 185.1 (1993): 283-289.
  • Gelkopf, Marc, and M. Sigal. “It is Not Enlough to Have them Laugh: Hostility, Anger, and Humor-Coping in Schizophrenic Patients.” Humor: International Journal of Humor Research 8.3 (1995): 273-284.
  • Gervais, Matthew and David Sloane Wilson. “The Evolution of Laughter and Humor: A Synthetic Approach.” Quarterly Review of Biology 80.4 (2005): 395-430.
  • Gierycha, Ewa, Rafal Milnera, and Andrzej Michalskia. “ERP Responses to Smile-Provoking Pictures.” Journal of Psychophysiology 19.2 (2005): 77-90.
  • Giles, H., and G. S. Oxford. “Towards a Multidimensional Theory of Laughter Causation and its Social Implications.” Bulletin of the British Psychological Society 23 (1970): 97-105.
  • Gilligan, B. “A Positive Coping Strategy: Humor in the Oncology Setting.” Professional Nurse 8.4 (1993): 231-233.
  • Gilman, Diane, and Joel Goodman. “Laughing Matters.” In Context 13 (Spring, 1986): 11-13.
  • Glasgow, R. D. V. Madness, Masks, and Laughter. Teaneck, NJ: Farleigh Dickinson University Press, 1995.
  • Glasgow, R. D. V. Split Down the Sides: On the Subject of Laughter. Lanham, MD: University Press of America, 1997.
  • Glen, P. “Initiating Shared Laughter in Multi-Party Conversations.” Western Journal of Speech Communication 53 (1989): 127-149.
  • Glenn, Phillip. Laughter in Interaction. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, 2003; reviewed by N. J. Enfield in Linguistics 400 (2005): 1195-1197; reviewed by Salvatore Attardo in HUMOR 18.4 (2005): 422-430.
  • Goldstein, Jeffrey H. “A Laugh a Day.” Sciences 22.6 (1982): 21-25.
  • Goldstein, Jeffrey H. “Therapeutic Effects of Laughter.” Handbook of Humor and Psychotherapy: Advances in the Clinical Use of Humor. Eds. William Fry and Waleed Salameh. Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Exchange, 1987, 1-20.
  • Goodman, Joel. “How to Get More Smileage Out of Your Life: Making Sense of Humor, Then Serving It.” Handbook of Research in Humor: Volume 2, Applied Studies Eds. P. E. McGhee, and J. H. Goldstein. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag, 1983, 1-21.
  • Goodrich, A. J., J. Henry, and D. W. Goodrich. “Laughter in Psychiatric Conferences: A Sociopsychiatric Analysis.” American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 24 (1954): 175-184.
  • Grant, Mary A. The Ancient Rhetorical Theories of the Laughable: The Greek Rhetoricians and Cicero. University of Wisconsin Studcies in Language and Literature 21. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin, 1924.
  • Gray, Frances. Women and Laughter. Charlottesville, VA: University Press of Virginia, 1994.
  • Gregory, J. C. The Nature of Laughter. New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace, and Company, 1924.
  • Greig, J. Y. T. The Psychology of Laughter and Comedy. New York, NY: Cooper Square, 1969.
  • Grimm, Reinhold, ed. Laughter Unlimited. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press, 1991.
  • Gronnerod, J. S. “On the Meanings and Uses of Laughter in Research Interviews: Relationships between Interviewed Men and a Woman Interviewer.” Young 12.1 (21004): 31-49.
  • Gross, E. “Laughter and Symbolic Interaction.” Symbolic Interaction 2 (1979): 111-112.
  • Grotjahn, Martin. “Beyond Laughter: A Summing Up.” Comedy: Meaning and Form. Ed. Robert W. Corrigan, San Francisco, CA: Chandler, 1965, 270-276.
  • Grumet, Gerald W. “Laughter: Nature’s Epileptoid Catharsis.” Psychological Reports 65 (1989): 1059-1078.
  • Gruner, Charles R. “Audiences’ Response to Jokes in Speeches With and Without, Recorded Laughs.” Psychological Reports 73.1 (1993): 347-350.
  • Gruner, Charles R. The Game of Humor: A Comprehensive Theory of Why We Laugh. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction, 1997.
  • Gruner, Charles R. Understanding Laughter: The Workings of Wit and Humor. Chicago: Nelson-Hall, 1978.
  • Gruner, Charles R., L. J. Pelletier, and M. A. Williams. “Evaluative Responses to Jokes in Informative Speech With and Without Laughter by an Audience: A Partial Replication.” Psychological Reports 74 (1994): 446.
  • Gurevich, Aaron. “Bakhtin and his Theory of Carnival.” A Cultural History of Humour: From Antiquity to the Present Day. Eds. Bremmer, Jan, and Herman Roodenburg. Cambridge, England: Polity Press, 1997, 54-60.
  • Gutwirth, Marcel. Laughing Matter: An Essay on the Comic. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press, 1993.
  • Hageseth, G. A Laughing Place: The Art and Psychology of Positive Humor in Love and Adversity Fort Collins, CO: Berwick, 1988.
  • Halliwell, S. “Greek Laughter and the Problem of the Absurd.” Arion 13.2 (2005): 121-146.
  • Hanly, Sheila. Peek-A-Boo! 101 Ways to Make a Baby Smile. New York, NY: D. K. Publishers, 1988.
  • Harral, Stewart. When It’s Laughter You’re After. Norman, OK: Univ of Oklahoma Press, 1962.
  • Hawakami, K. K. Takai-Kawakami, M. Tomonaga, J. Suzuki, T. Kusaka, and T. Okay. “Origins of Smiles and Laughter: A Preliminary Study.” Early Human Development (2005).
  • Hayashi, T., O. Urayama, K. Kawai, K. Hayashi, S. Iwanaga, M. Ohta, T. Saito, and K. Murakami. “Laughter Regulates Gene Expression in Patients with Type 2 Diabetes.” Psychother. Psychosom. 75.1 (2006): 62-65.
  • Hayworth, D. “Social Origin and Function of Laughter.” Psychological Review 35 (1928): 367-384.
  • Hennenlotter, A., U. Schroeder, P. Erhard, F. Castrop, B. Haslinger, D. Stoecker, K. W. Lange, and A. O. Caballos-Bauman. “A Common Neural Basis for Receptive and Expressive Communicatoin of Pleasant Facial Affect.” Neuroimage 26.2 (2005): 581-591.
  • Herth, K. “Laughter: A Nursing Treatment.” American Journal of Nursing 84 (1984): 991-992.
  • Hertzler, Joyce O. Laughter: A Socio-Scientific Analysis. New York, NY: Exposition Press, 1970.
  • Holden, Robert. Laughter–The Best Medicine. London, England: Thorsons, 1995.
  • Holland, Norman N. Laughing: A Psychology of Humor. Ithaca, NY: Cornell Univ Press, 1982.
  • Holme, Bryan. A Present of Laughter. NY: Viking, 1982.
  • Hong, W. “Learning through Laughter: The Use of Cartoons in Business Chinese.” Journal of Language for International Business 15.1 (2004): 100-116.
  • Humann, Ursula. “Der Witz als Waffe: Lachen and Humor in der Jüdischen Tradition.” Tribüne 126 (1993): 179-187.
  • Hudak, D., A. Dale, M. Hudak, and D. DeGood. “Effects of Humorous Stimuli and Sense of Humor on Discomfort.” Psychological Reports 69.3 (1991): 779-786.
  • Hutcheson, Francis. Reflections on Laughter. New York, NY: Garland, 1971.
  • Ishigami, S, A. Nakajima, M. Tanno, T. Matsuzaki, H. Suzuki, and S. Yoshino. “Effects of Mirthful Laughter on Growth Hormone, IGF-1 and Substance P in Patients with Rheumatoid Arthritis.” Clin. Exp. Rheumatol. 23.5 (2005): 651-657.
  • Jefferson, Gail. “An Exercise in the Transcription and Analysis of Laughter.” Handbook of Discourse Analysis, Volume 3: Discourse and Dialogue. Ed. T. A. van Dijk. London, England: Academic Press, 1985, 25-34.
  • Jefferson, Gail. “On the Organization of Laughter in Talk about Troubles.” Structures of Social Action: Studies in Conversational Analysis. Eds. J. Atkinson and J. Heritage. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, 1984, 346-369.
  • Jefferson, Gail. “A Technique for Inviting Laughter and its Subsequent Acceptance/Declination.” Everyday Language: Studies in Ethnomethodology. Ed. G. Psathas. New York, NY: Irvington, 1979, 79-96.
  • Jenkins, Ron. Subversive Laughter: the Liberating Power of Comedy. New York, NY: Free Press, 1994.
  • Johnson, Helen. “Counteracting Performaitivity in Schools: The Case for Laughter as a Qualitative and Redemptive Indicator.” International Journal of Children’s Spirituality 10.1 (2005): 81-96.
  • Joubert, Laurent. Treatise on Laughter. Trans. Gregory David de Rocher. Birmingham, AL: University of Alabama Press, 1980.
  • Juni, Samuel, and Bernard Katz. “Self-Effacing Wit as a Response to Oppression: Dynamics in Ethnic Humor.” Journal of General Psychology Special Issue on Humor and Laughter Eds. Mahony, Diana L. and Louis G. Lippman.128.2 (2001): 117-119.
  • Kaplan, L. “Suspense, Para-Science and Laughter.” Sub-Stance 71-71 (1993): 306-314.
  • Karassev, Leonid V. Filosofia Smekha/Philosophy of Laughter. Moscow, Russia: R.G.G.U, 1996.
  • Kehl, D. G. “Varieties of Risible Experience: Grades of Laughter in Modern American Literature.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research 13.4 (2000): 279-395.
  • Kelly, K. “Laughter: A Hearty Har-Har.” US News and World Report (March 21, 2005): 138-155.
  • Killeen, M. “Clinical Clowning: Humor in Hospice Care.” American Journal of Hospice and Palliative Care 8.3 (1991): 23-27.
  • Kimata, H. “Reduction of Allergic Responses in Atopic Infants by Mother’s Laughter.” European Journal of Clinical Investigation 34.9 (2004): 645-646.
  • Kotani, K. “Socio-Psychological Activities Associated with Laughter in Older Japanese Females.” Arch. Med. Res 37.1 (2006): 186-187.
  • Kraut, R. E., and R. E. Johnston. “Social and Emotional Messages of Smiling: An Ethological Approach.” Journal of Personality Social Psychology. 37 (1979): 1539-1553.
  • Kreitler, H., and S. Kreitler. “Dependence of Laughter on Cognitive Strategies.” Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 16 (April, 1970): 163-177.
  • Kuhn, C. C. “The Stages of Laughter.” Journal of Nursing Jocularity 4.2 (1994): 34-35.
  • Kuipers, Giselinde. “Where Was King Kong When We Needed Him?: Public Discourse, Digital Disaster Jokes, and the Functions of Laughter after 9/11.” Journal of American and Comparative Cultures 28.1 (2005): 70-84.
  • Kundera, Milan. The Book of Laughter and Forgetting. New York, NY: Penguin, 1980.
  • Kushner, B. “Laughter as Materiel: The Mobilization of Comedy in Japan’s Fifteen-Year War.” International History Review 26.2 (21004): 300-330.
  • LaFrance, M. “Felt Versus Feigned Funniness: Issues in Smiling and Laughing.” Handbook of Humor Research I. Eds. P. G. McGhee, and J. H. Goldstein. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag, 1983, 1-12.
  • Labott, S., and R. Martin. “The Stress-Moderating Effects of Weeping and Humor.” Journal of Human Stress 13.4 (1987): 159-164.
  • Lahue, Kalton C. World of Laughter. Norman, OK: University of Oklahoma Press, 1966.
  • Lally, Steven. “Laugh Your Stress Away.” Prevention. June, 1991: 50ff.
  • Lamb, Chris. “The Popularity of O. J. Simpson Jokes: The More We Know, the More We Laugh.” Journal of Popular Culture 28.1 (1994): 223-232.
  • Lambert, R., and N. Lambert. “The Effects of Humor on Secretory Immunoglobulin A Levels in School-Aged Children.” Pediatric Nursing 21.1 (1995): 16-19.
  • Lampert, Martin D., and Susan Ervin-Tripp. “Risky Laughter: Teasing and Self-Directed Joking among Male and Female Friends.” Journal of Pragmatics, 38.1 (2006): 51-72.
  • Laura, Ronald S., and Bob Wolff. “Not Just for Laughs: Humor Can Relieve Stress and Prolong Life.” Muscle and Fitness. December, 1992: 148ff.
  • Leventhal, H., and W. Mace. “The Effect of Laughter on Evaluation of a Slapstick Movie.” Journal of Personality 38 (1970): 16-30.
  • Levi, Primo. “Ritual and Laughter.” Other People’s Trades. New York, NY: Summit Books, 1989.
  • Lieber, D. B. “Laughter and Humor in Critical Care. Dimens Crit Care Nurs 5.3 (1986): 162-70.
  • Light, K. Humor as a Coping Strategy: Its Relationship to Role Strain in Women. M.A. Thesis. Tampa, FL: University of South Florida, 1997.
  • Lindvall, Terry. Surprised by Laughter. Nashville, TN: T. Nelson, 1996.
  • Lipman, Steve. Laughter in Hell: The Use of Humor during the Holocaust. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson, 1991.
  • Lippitt, John. “Nietzsche, Zarathustra and the Status of Laughter.” British Journal of Aesthetics 32.1 (1992).
  • Littleton, J. “Learning to Laugh, and Laughing to Learn.” Montessori Life 10 (1998): 42-44.
  • Long, Patricia. “Laugh and Be Well?” Psychology Today 21.10 (1987): 28-29.
  • Loomans, D., and K. J. Kolberg. The Laughing Classroom: Everyone’s Guide to Teaching with Humor and Play. Tiburon, CA: H. J. Kramer, 1993.
  • Lowe, G., and S. B. Taylor. “Relationship between Laughter and Weekly Alcohol Consumption.” Psychological Reports 72.3 (1993): 1210
  • Ludovici, Anthony M. The Secret of Laughter. London, England: Constable Press, 1932.
  • Lundell, Torborg. “An Experiential Exploration of Why Men and Women Laugh.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research 6.3 (1993): 299-318.
  • McAdams, D. P., R. J. Jackson, and C. Kirshmit. “Looking, Laughing, and Smiling in Dyads as a Function of Intimacy Motivations and Reciprocity.” Journal of Personality. 52.3 (1984): 261-273.
  • McGhee, Paul E. The Laughter Remedy: Health, Healing, and the Amuse System. Randolph, New Jersey: pAUL mCgHEE, 1991.
  • MacHovec, F. “Humor in Therapy.” Psychotherapy in Private Practice 9.1 (1991): 25-33.
  • McMahon, C., A. Mahmud, and J. Freely. “Taking Blood Pressure–No Laughing Matter!” Blood Pressur Monitor 10.2 (2005): 109-110.
  • Machline, Vera Cecelia. “The Legend Behind the Epithet `Sardonic Laugh,’ 2000 Ans de Rire: Permanence et Modernite ed. Mongi Medina. Besançons, France: Presses Universitaires France-Comtoises, 2002, 77-86.
  • Mager, M., and P. A. Cabe. “Is Propensity to Laugh Equivalent to Sense of Humor?” Psychological Reports 66.3 (1990): 737-738.
  • Mahony, Diana L., and M. D. Corson. “Light-Mindedness versus Lightheartedness: Conflicting Conceptions of Laughter among Latter-Day Saints.” BYU Studies 42.2 (2003): 115-129.
  • Mahony, Diana L., W. Jeffrey Burroughs, and Arron C. Hieatt. “The Effects of Laughter on Discomfort Thresholds: Does Expectation Become Reality?” Journal of General Psychology “Special Issue on Humor and Laughter” Eds. Mahony, Diana L. and Louis G. Lippman.128.2 (2001): 217-226.
  • Mahony, Diana L. and Louis G. Lippman, eds. “Introduction to the Special Issue on `Humor and Laughter.'” Journal of General Psychology 128.2 (2001): 117-119.
  • Mallett, Jane. “Humour and Laughter Therapy.” The Nurses’ Handbook of Complementary Therapies. London, England: Churchill-Livingston, 1995, 109-117.
  • Mallett, Jane. “The Use of Humour and Laughter in Patient Care.” British Journal of Nursing. 2 (1993): 172-175.
  • Mandel, Siegfried. “The Laughter of Nordic and Celtic Irish Tricksters.” Fabula 23.1-2 (1982): 35-47.
  • Marmysz, John. Laughing at Nothing: Humor as a Response to Nihilism. Albany, NY: SUNY Press, 2003.
  • Martin, G. Neil, and Colin D. Gray. “The Effects of Audience Laughter on Men’s and Women’s Responses to Humor.” Journal of Social Psychology 136.2 (1996): 221-231.
  • Martin, Rod A. “Humor, Laughter, and Physical Health: Methodological Issues and Research Findings.” Psychological Bulletin 127.4 (2001): 504-519.
  • Martin, Rod, N. Kuiper, J. Olinger, and K. Dance. “Humor, Coping with Stress, Self Concept, and Psychological Well-being.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research 6.1 (1993): 89-104.
  • Martin, Rod, and Herbert Lefcourt. “Sense of Humor as a Moderator of the Relation between Stressors and Mood.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45 (1983): 1313-1324.
  • Meyer, M., S. Zysset, D. Y. von Cramon, and K. Alter. “Distinct fMRI Responses to Laughter, Speech, and Sounds along the Human Peri-Sylvian Cortex.” Cognitive Brain Research 24.2 (2005): 291-306.
  • Mills, Letha. “Laughter is the Best Medicine.” Active Years 14 (2002): 74-77.
  • Milner, George B. “Homo Ridens: Towards a Semiotic Theory of Humor and Laughter.” Semiotica 5 (1972): 1-30.
  • Miyamoto, Lance. “Why Laughing is Good for You.” Science Digest. June, 1981, 27.
  • Monro, D. H. Argument of Laughter. Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press, 1963.
  • Morreall, John. “Humor and Laughter.” Encyclopedia of U.S. Popular Culture. Forthcoming.
  • Morreall, John. “Laughter.” Encyclopaedia Britannica, Japanese Edition. Forthcoming.
  • Morreall, John. “Laughter, Suddenness, and Pleasure.” Dialogue: Canadian Philosophical Review 23 (1984): 689-694.
  • Morreall, John. “A New Theory of Laughter.” The Philosophy of Laughter and Humor. Ed. John Morreall. Albany, NY: SUNY Press, 1987, 128-38.
  • Morreall, John. Taking Laughter Seriously. Albany, NY: SUNY Press, 1983.
  • Mowrer, Donald E. “A Case Study of Perceptual and Acoustic Features of an Infant’s First Laugh Utterances.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research 7.2 (1994): 139-156.
  • Mueller, Rolph, and Guenter Braus. “Laughter,” Special Issue of High Quality: Magazine of Design and Printing 22.1 (1992). Heidelberg, Germany: Rosi Pluschke-Moser; NOTE: There is also a German version.
  • Mulkay, Michael, Colin Clark, and Trevor Pinch. “Laughter and the Profit Motive: The Use of Humor in a Photographic Shop.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research. 6.2 (1993): 163-194.
  • Mulkay, Michael, and Gerard Howe. “Laughter for Sale.” The Sociological Review 42.3 (1994): 481-500.
  • Nasir, U. M., S. Iwanaga, Ahmn Nabi, O. Urayama, K. Hayashi, T. Hayashi, K. Kawai, A. Sultana, K. Murakami, and F. Suzuki. “Laughter Therapy Modulates the Parameters of Renin-Angiotensin System in Patients with Type 2 Diabetes.” International Journal of Molecular Medicine 16.6 (2005): 1077-1082.
  • Nerhardt, G. “Humor and Inclination to Laugh: Emotional Reactions to Stimuli of Different Divergence from a Range of Expectancy.” Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 11 (1970): 185-95.
  • Neuendorf, Kimberley, and Tom Fennell. “A Social Facilitation View of the Generation of Humor and Mirth Reactions: Effects of a Laugh Track.” Central States Speech Journal 39.1 (1988): 37-48.
  • Nichols, Stephen G. “Laughter as Gesture: Hilarity and the Anti-Sublime.” Neohelicon 32.2 (2005): 375-389.
  • Niebylski, Dianna C. Humoring Resistance: Laughter and the Excessive Body in Contemporary Women’s Fiction, Albany, NY: State University of New York Press, 2004.
  • Niles, R. “Wigs, Laughter, and Subversion: Charles Busch and Strategies of Drag Performance.” Journal of Homosexuality 46.3-4 (2004): 35-54.
  • Nilsen, Alleen Pace, and Don L. F. Nilsen. “Laughter and Smiles.” Encyclopedia of 20th Century American Humor. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 2000, 184-186.
  • Nilsen, Don L. F. “Laughter and Smiling.” Humor Scholarship: A Research Bibliography. Westport, CT: Greenwood, 1993, 1-5.
  • Nwokah, Eva E. “Giggle Time in the Infant/Toddler Classroom: Learning and Connecting Through Shared Humor and Laughter.” Focus on Infants and Toddlers 16.2 (2003): 1-8.
  • Nwokah, Evangeline, and Alan Fogel. “Laughter in Mother-Infant Emotional Communication.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research. 6.2 (1993): 137-162.
  • O’Donnell-Trujillo, Nick, and Katherine Adams. “Heheh in Conversation: Some Coordinating Accomplishments of Laughter.” The Western Journal of Speech Communications 47 (Spring 1983): 175-191.
  • Okun, M. S., D. Bowers, U. Springer, N. A. Shapira, D. Malone, A. R. Rezai, B. Nuttin, K. M. Heilman, R. J. Morecraft, S. A. Rasmussen, B. D. Greenberg, K. D. Foote, and W. K. Goodman.” Neurocase 10.4 (2004): 271-279.
  • Osaka, N., and M. Osaka. “Striatal Reward Areas Activated by Implicit Laughter Induced by Mimic Words in Humans: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study.” Neuroreport 16.15 (2005): 1621-1624.
  • Overeem, Sebastian, Walter Taal, E. Öcal Gezici, Gert Jan Lammers, and J. Gert van Dijk. “Is Motor Inhibition during Laughter due to Emotional or Respiratory Influences?” Pscychophysiology 41.2 (2004): 254ff.
  • Owren, M. J., and J-A Bachorowski. “Reconsidering the Evolution of Nonlinguistic Communication: The Case of Laughter.” Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 27.3 (2003): 183-200.
  • Parse, R. R. “Laughing and Health–A Study Using Parse Research Method.” Nursing Science Quarterly 123.2 (1994): 129-145.
  • Paskind, J. “Effects of Laughter on Muscle Tone.” Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 28 (1932): 623-628.
  • Pasquali, Elaine Anne. “Learning to Laugh: Humor as Therapy.” Journal of Psychosocial Nursing 28.3 (March, 1990): 31-35.
  • Paul, William. Laughing Screaming: Modern Hollywood Horror and Comedy. New York, NY: Columbia University Press, 1994.
  • Pepper, Melissa. “In Outer Space No One Can Hear Your Laughter.” Australian Journal of Comedy 2 (1995): 101-114.
  • Peter, Laurence, and Bill Dana. The Laughter Prescription. New York, NY: Ballentine, 1982.
  • Pfeifer, Karl. “Laughter and Pleasure.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research 7 (1994): 157-172.
  • Pfeifer, Karl. “Laughter, Freshness and Titillation.” Inquiry 40 (1997): 307-322.
  • Pfeifer, Karl. “From Locus Neoclassicus to Locus Rattus: Notes on Laughter, Comprehensiveness, and Titillation. ” Res Cogitans 3 (2006) 29-46.
  • Pfeifer, Karl. “More on Morreall on Laughter.” Dialogue 26 (1987): 161-166.
  • Piddington, Ralph. The Psychology of Laughter. New York, NY: Gamut Press, 1963.
  • Plessner, Helmuth. Laughing and Crying. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press, 1970.
  • Podlichak, Walter. “Fun, Funny, Fun–Of Humor and Laughter.” Humor: International Journal of Humor Research. 5.4 (1992): 375-396.
  • Polio, Howard R., Rodney Mers, and William Lucchesi. “Humor, Laughter, and Smiling: Some Preliminary Observations of Funny Behaviors.” The Psychology of Humor. Eds. Jeffrey Goldstein and Paul McGhee. NY: Academic Press, 1972,2 211-42.
  • Porteous, Janice. “Humor as a Process of Defense: The Evolution of Laughing.” Humor: International Journal of Humor Research 1.1 (1988): 63-80.
  • Powell, B. S. “Laughter and Healing: The Use of Humor in Hospitals Treating Children.” J Assoc Care Child Hosp 3.2 (1974): 10-16.
  • Poyatos, F. “Many Voices of Laughter–A New Audible-Visual Paralinguistic Approach.” Semiotica 93.1-2 (1993): 61-81.
  • Prerost, F. “Presentation of Humor and Facilitation of a Relaxation Response among Internal and External Scores on Rotter’s Scale.” Psychological Reports 72 (1993): 1248-1250.
  • Prerost, F. “Use of Humor and Guided Imagery in Therapy to Alleviate Stress.” Journal of Mental Health Counseling 10.1 (1988): 16-22.
  • Propp, Vladimir. “Ritual Laughter in Folklore.” Theory and History of Folklore. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota, 1984.
  • Provine, Robert R. “Contagious Laughter: Laughter is a Sufficient Stimulus for Laughs and Smiles.” Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30.1 (1992): 1-4.
  • Provine, Robert R. “Contageous Yawning and Laughter.” Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture. Eds. C. M. Heyes, and B. G. Galef. New York, NY: Academic Press, 1996.
  • Provine, Robert R. “The Laughing Species.” Natural History December, 2000, 72-77.
  • Provine, Robert R. “Laughing, Tickling, and the Evolution of Speech and Self.” Current Directions in Psychological Science 13.6 (2004): 215-218.
  • Provine, Robert R. “Laughter.” American Scientist 84.1 (1996): 38-48.
  • Provine, Robert R. “Laughter: A Novel Tool for Understanding Vocal Production, Perceptions, and Social Behavior.” American Scientist 84.1 (1996): 38-40
  • Provine, Robert R. Laughter: A Scientific Investigation. New York, NY: Harmondsworth/Penguin, 2001.
  • Provine, Robert R. “Laughter Punctuates Speech: Linguistic, Social and Gender Contexts of Laughter.” Ethology 95.4 (1993): 291-298.
  • Provine, Robert R., and Kenneth R. Fischer. “Laughing, Smiling, and Talking: Relation to Sleeping and Social Context in Humans.” Ethology 83 (1989): 295-305.
  • Provine, Robert R., and Yvonne L. Young. “Laughter: A Stereotyped Human Vocalization.” Ethology 89 (1991): 115-124.
  • Rankin, A. M., and P. J. Philip. “An Epidemic of Laughing in the Bukoba District of Tanganyika. Central African Journal of Medicine 9 (1963): 167-170.
  • Ransohoff, R. “Some Observations on Humor and Laughter in Young Adolescent Girls.” Journal of Youth and Adolescence 4 (1975): 155-170.
  • Raskin, Victor. “Better to Laugh: Linking Humor, Creativity, and Intelligence.” The World and I (June, 1992): 658-660.
  • Repplier, Agnes. In Pursuit of Laughter. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin, 1936.
  • Rothbart, Mary K. “Incongruity, Problem-Solving and Laughter.” Humor and Laughter: Theory, Research, and Applications. Eds. Antony J. Chapman, and Hugh C. Foot. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction, 1996, 37-54.
  • Rothbart, Mary K. “Laughter in Young Children.” Psycholigical Bulletin 80.3 (1973): 247-256.
  • Ruch, Willibald. “Commentary.” Charles Darwins’ Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals (3rd Edition). Ed. Paul Ekman. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 1998.
  • Ruch, Willibald. “Extraversion, Alcohol, and Enjoyment.” What the Face Reveals: Basic and Applied Studies of Spontaneous Expression Using the Facial Action Coding System. Eds. Paul Ekman, and E. L. Rosenberg. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press, 1997, 112-130.
  • Ruch, Willibald. “The FACS in Humor Research.” What the Face Reveals: Basic and Applied Studies of Spontaneous Expression Using the Facial Action Coding System. Eds. Paul Ekman, and E. L. Rosenberg. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press, 1997, 109-111.
  • Ruch, Willibald. “Laughter and Temperament.” What the Face Reveals: Basic and Applied Studies of Spontaneous Expression Using the Facial Action Coding System. Eds. P. Ekman, and E. L. Rosenberg. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press, 1997, 131-132.
  • Ruch, Willibald, and Lambert Deckers. “Do Extroverts Like to Laugh? An Analysis of the Situational Humor Response Questionnaire.” European Journal of Personality 7.4 (1993): 211-220.
  • Ruch, Willibald, and P. Ekman. “The Expressive Pattern of Laughter.” Emotion, Qualia, and Consciousness. Tokyo, Japan: Word Scientific Publisher, 2001.
  • Russell, Roy E. Life, Mind and Laughter: A Theory of Laughter. Chicago, IL: Adams Press, 1987.
  • Russell, Roy E. “Understanding Laughter in Terms of Basic Perceptual and Response Patterns.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research 9.1 (1996): 39-56.
  • Rutter, Jason. Stand-Up as Interaction: Performance and Audience in Comedy Venues. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation. Salford, England: University of Salford, 1997.
  • Ruxton, J., and M. Hesler. “Humor: Assessment and Interventions.” Clinical Gerontologist 7.1 (1987): 13-21.
  • Salamone, Frank A. “Laughin’ Louie: An Analysis of Louis Armstrong’s Record and its Relationship to African-American Musical Humor.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research 15.1 (2002): 47-64.
  • Sanders, Barry. Laughter as Subversive History. Boston, MA: Beacon Press, 1995.
  • Sanders, T. “Controllable Laughter: Managing Sex Work through Humour.” Sociology 38.2 (2004): 273-291.
  • Satow, R. “Three Perspectives on Humor and Laughing–Classical, Object, Relations and Self Psychology.” Group 15.4 (1991): 242-245.
  • Schaeffer, Neil. The Art of Laughter. New York: Columbia Univ Press, 1981.
  • Scherberger, L. “The Janus-Faced Shaman: The Role of Laughter in Sickness and Healing among the Makushi.” Anthropology and Humanism 30.1 (2005): 55-69.
  • Schickel, Richard. The Shape of Laughter. Boston, MA: New York Graphics Society, 1974.
  • Schopenhauer, Arthur. “On the Theory of the Ludicrous.” The World as will and Idea. Trans. R. B. Haldane and John Kemp. London, England: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1907.
  • Schulman, N. M. “Laughing across the Color Barrier: In Living Color–Satirizing the Stereotypes–Racial Generalization as a Basis for Comedy.” Journal of Popular Film and Television 20.1 (1992): 2-7.
  • Scruton, Roger. “Laughter.” The Philosophy of Laughter and Humor. Ed. John Morreall. Albany, NY: SUNY, 1987, 156-171.
  • Scruton, Roger. “Laughter.” Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society 56 (1982).
  • Shapiro, Rebecca. R. D. V. Glasgow: Madness, Masks, and Laughter. Teaneck, NJ: Farleigh Dickinson University Press, 1995.
  • Sheldon, S. T. “Tickle.” Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology. 50 (2004): 93-97.
  • Shoji, Osamu. “Study of Dr. Sutorius’ Theory Concerning Laughter.” The Journal of Ryutsu Keizai University 34.3 (2000): 51-55.
  • Simon, Jolene. “Humor and the Older Adult: Implications for Nursing.” Journal of Advanced Nursing Practice 13 (1988): 441-446.
  • Simon, Jolene. “Humor Techniques for Oncology Nurses.” Oncology Nursing Forum 16.5 (19789): 667-670.
  • Smith, J. “The Frenzy of the Audible: Pleasure, Authenticity, and Recorded Laughter.” Television and New Media 6.1 (2005): 23-48.
  • Smith, Ken. “Laughing at the Way We See: The Role of Visual Organizing Principles in Cartoon Humor.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research 9.1 (1996): 19-38.
  • Solomon, Robert. “Are the Three Stooges Funny? Soitainly! (or When is it OK to Laugh?).” Ethics and Values in the Information Age. Eds. Joel Rudinow and Anthony Graybosch. New York, NY: Wadsworth, 2002.
  • Spencer, Herbert. “The Physiology of Laughter.” MaclMillan’s Magazine 1 (1860).
  • Spitz, R. “The Smiling Response: A Contribution to the Ontogenesis of Social Relations.” General Psychology Monograph 34 (1946): 57-125.
  • Strack, F., L. Martin, and S. Strepper. “Inhibiting and Facilitating Conditions of the Human Smile: A Nonobtrusive Test of the Facial-Feedback Hypothesis.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54 (1988): 768-777.
  • Stearns, F. R. Laughing: Physiology, Pathology, Psychology, Pathopsychology and Development. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas, 1972.
  • Stebbins, Robert A. The Laugh-Makers: Stand-Up Comedy as Art, Business, and Life-Style. Montreal: McGill-Queen’s University Press, 1990.
  • Sterling, Philip. Laughing on the Outside. NY: Grosset and Dunlap, 1965.
  • Stone, Judith. “Laugh and Your Whole Cardiovascular System Laughs with You–Not to Mention Your Stress Hormones.” In Health. 5 (January, 1992): 52-55.
  • Stroufe, L. A., and E. Waters. “The Ontogenesis of Smiling and Laughter: A Perspective on the Organization of Development in Infancy.” Psychological Review 83.3 (1976): 173-189.
  • Stroufe, L. A., and J. P. Wunsch. “The Development of Laughter in the First Year of Life.” Child Development 43 (1972): 1326-1344.
  • Sully, J. An Essay on Laughter. New York, NY: Longmans/Green, 1902.
  • Sumitsuji, N., and Y. Takemura. “Electromyography Counting of the Numbers of Times of the Laughing Act in Man.” Electromyography 1 (1971): 55-60.
  • Svebak, Sven. “Three Attitude Dimensions of Sense of Humor as Predictors of Laughter.” Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 15 (1974): 185-190.
  • Svebak, Sven, and Michael J. Apter. “Laughter: An Empirical Test of Some Reversal Theory Hypotheses.” Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 28 (1987): 189-198.
  • Swabey, Mary Collins. “Comic Laughter: A Philosophical Essay.” New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, 1961.
  • Tamblyn, Doni. Laugh and Learn: 95 Ways to Use Humor for More Effective Teaching and Training. New York, NY: AMACON (American Management Association), 2003.
  • Tarantili, V. V. D. J. Halazonetis, and M. N. Spyropoulos. “The Spontaneous Smile in Dynamic Motion.” American Journal of Dentofacial Orthop 128.1 (2005): 8-15.
  • Taylor, Paul. “Laughter and Joking–The Structural Axis.” It’s a Funny Thing, Humour. Eds. Antony Chapman and Hugh Foot. NY: Pergamon, 1977, 385-90.
  • Thorson, James A. “Is Propensity to Laugh Equivalent to Sense of Humor?” Psychological Reports 66.3 (1990): 737-738.
  • Toren, C. “Laughter and Truth in Fiji: What We May Learn from a Joke.” Oceania 75.3 (2005): 268-283.
  • Trice, A., and J. Price. “Joking under the Drill: A Validity Study of the Coping Humor Scale.” Journal of Social Behavior and Personality 1.2 (1986): 265-266.
  • Trieber, Roz. Live Life Laughing: An Innovative and Imaginative Approach to Living a Healthier, Happier, and More Prosperous Life. Owings Mills, MD: Trieber Associates, 2000.
  • Vaid, Jyotsna. “Do Those Who Laugh Last? The Evolution of Humor.” Evolution of the Psyche. Eds. D. Rosen, and M. Luebbert. Westport, CT: Greenwood, 1999.
  • Vaid, Jyotsna. “Laughter and Humor.” Encyclopedia of the Human Brain. Ed. V. S. Ramachandran. Academic Press, 2000.
  • Vaid, Jyotsna. “Laughter and Humor.” Oxford Companion to the Body Eds. C. Blakemore and S. Jennett. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press, 2001, 426-427.
  • Vaid, Jyotsna, and J. B. Kobler. “Laughing Matters: Toward a Structural and Neural Account.” Brain and Cognition 42 (2000): 139-141.
  • Van Hoof, J. A. R. A. M. “A Comparative Approach to the Phylogeny of Laughter and Smiling.” Non-Verbal Communication. Ed. R. A. Hinds. Cambridge, England: University Press.
  • Vettin, Julia, and Dietmar Todt. “Laughter in Conversation: Features of Occurence and Acoustic Structure.” Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 28.2 (2004): 93-115
  • Viktoroff, David. Introduction à la Psycho-Sociologie du Rire. Paris, France: Presses Universitaires de France, 1953.
  • Wallace, Earle Stegner. Remembering Laughter. Boston, MA: Little Brown, 1937.
  • Waller, Bridget M., and Robin I. M. Dunbar. “Differential Behavioural Effects of Silent Bared Teeth Display and Relaxed Open Mouth Display in Chimpanzees (Pan Troglodytes).” Ethology 3.2 (2005): 129-142.
  • Weeks, Mark C. “Laughter, Desire, and Time.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research. 15.4 (2002): 383-400.
  • Weisfeld, G. E. “Adaptive Value of Humor and Laughter.” Ethology and Sociobiology 14.2 (1993): 141-169.
  • Weiskrantz, L. J., Elliott, and C. Darlington. “Preliminary Observations on Tickling Oneself.” Nature 230 (1970): 598-599.
  • Westbrook, Kathy Grant. “Laugh It Up: Carolina Ha Ha Prescribes a Healthy Dose of Humor to Combat Pain and Stress and to Help Folks Feel More Positive.” Our State: Down Home in North Carolilna, (January, 2005): 62-64.
  • Whistler, Laurence. Laughter and the Urn: The Life of Rex Whistler. NY: Wiedenfeld and Nicolson, 1985.
  • White, Sabina, and Andrew Winzelberg. “Laughter and Stress.” Humor: International Journal of Humor Research. 5.4 (1992): 343-356.
  • White, Sabina, and P. Camarena. “Laughter as a Stress Reducer in Small Groups.” HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research 2.1 (1989): 73-79.
  • Williams, H. “Humor and Healing: Therapeutic Effects in Geriatrics.” Gerontion 1.3 (1986): 14-17.
  • Willman, J. M. “An Analysis of Humor and Laughter.” American Journal of Psychology 53 (1940).
  • Wise, B. “Comparison of Immune Response to Mirth and to Distress in Women at Risk for Recurrent Breast Cancer.” Dissertation Abstracts International 49.7 (1989): 2918.
  • Wolosin, R. J. “Cognitive Similarity and Group Laughter.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 32 (1975): 505-509.
  • Wooten, Patty. Compassionate Laughter: Jest for Your Health. New York, NY: Commune-a-Key, 1996.
  • Ziegler, J. “Immune System May Benefit from the Ability to Laugh.” Journal of the National Cancer Institute 87.5 (1995): 342-343.
  • Zijderveld, Anton C. “The Sociology of Humour and Laughter.” Current Sociology 31.3 (1983): 1-103.
  • Zijderveld, Anton C. “Trend Report on the Sociology of Humor and Laughter.” Current Sociology 31.3 (1983): 1-59.

Share This Story, Choose Your Platform!

About the author: sebastian gendry.

research essay on laughter

Related Posts

The Healing Power of Laughter

From Chuckles to Healing: 2023’s Comprehensive Guide to Laughter Studies

research essay on laughter

Science Of Laughter: The Ultimate Cheat Sheet

research essay on laughter

How To Manage Stress The Fun Way

Laughter News

Laughter, Exercise and Dialysis

Therapeutic benefits of laughter in mental health: a theoretical review.

research essay on laughter

Why Laughter Wellness is an Effective Recovery Activity for Emotional Labour

Leave a comment cancel reply.

Laughter Yoga International

  • Knowledge Center
  • Research Articles
  • The Effect of Laughter Yo..

The Effect of Laughter Yoga on the Quality of Life of Elderly Nursing Home Residents

The Effect of Laughter Yoga on the Quality of Life of Elderly Nursing Home Residents

Department of Psychology, Faculty of Literature and Humanities, University of Guilan, Rasht, Iran ( 2019 )

Background: Old age is associated with the physical and psychological problems that affect the Quality of Life (QoL). Objectives: This study aimed to investigate the effect of Laughter Therapy (LT) on the QoL of elderly people in nursing homes.

Materials & Methods: This was a quasi-experimental study with pre-test and post-test with the control and experimental group and one-month follow-up conducted in 2017. The statistical population consisted of 44 elderly people in the Mehr Aein nursing home in Astaneh-ye Ashrafiyeh City, Iran. Based on the inclusion criteria, 32 old people were selected and randomly assigned to experimental (n=16) and control (n=16) groups. The experimental group received an interventional program consisting of Madan Kataria Laughter Therapy, showing comic films, and reading comic books for one month, over 12 sessions of 100 minutes. After the end of laughter intervention, the elderly of both groups responded to the elderly quality of life questionnaire (LIPAD). The obtained data were analyzed using covariance analysis of repeated measures.

Results: There was a significant difference between the two groups with regard to their QoL (P<0.01). That is, the adjusted mean of the experimental group in the QoL was significantly higher than the mean of the control group (P<0.01). Also, in the experimental group, the mean follow-up score was higher than the mean score of the pre-test, which indicates the stability of the intervention effects after one month (P<0.01). 

Conclusion: Laughter Therapy intervention has a positive effect on improving the QoL of the elderly. Therefore, it is necessary that authorities consider this program at nursing home centers as a complementary method besides the existing treatments for reducing the mental health problems and improving the QoL of the elderly.

Click here to see original research paper  https://cjns.gums.ac.ir/article-1-254-en.pdf

Basic Learning Course

Help | Advanced Search

Computer Science > Computation and Language

Title: mapping the increasing use of llms in scientific papers.

Abstract: Scientific publishing lays the foundation of science by disseminating research findings, fostering collaboration, encouraging reproducibility, and ensuring that scientific knowledge is accessible, verifiable, and built upon over time. Recently, there has been immense speculation about how many people are using large language models (LLMs) like ChatGPT in their academic writing, and to what extent this tool might have an effect on global scientific practices. However, we lack a precise measure of the proportion of academic writing substantially modified or produced by LLMs. To address this gap, we conduct the first systematic, large-scale analysis across 950,965 papers published between January 2020 and February 2024 on the arXiv, bioRxiv, and Nature portfolio journals, using a population-level statistical framework to measure the prevalence of LLM-modified content over time. Our statistical estimation operates on the corpus level and is more robust than inference on individual instances. Our findings reveal a steady increase in LLM usage, with the largest and fastest growth observed in Computer Science papers (up to 17.5%). In comparison, Mathematics papers and the Nature portfolio showed the least LLM modification (up to 6.3%). Moreover, at an aggregate level, our analysis reveals that higher levels of LLM-modification are associated with papers whose first authors post preprints more frequently, papers in more crowded research areas, and papers of shorter lengths. Our findings suggests that LLMs are being broadly used in scientific writings.

Submission history

Access paper:.

  • HTML (experimental)
  • Other Formats

license icon

References & Citations

  • Google Scholar
  • Semantic Scholar

BibTeX formatted citation

BibSonomy logo

Bibliographic and Citation Tools

Code, data and media associated with this article, recommenders and search tools.

  • Institution

arXivLabs: experimental projects with community collaborators

arXivLabs is a framework that allows collaborators to develop and share new arXiv features directly on our website.

Both individuals and organizations that work with arXivLabs have embraced and accepted our values of openness, community, excellence, and user data privacy. arXiv is committed to these values and only works with partners that adhere to them.

Have an idea for a project that will add value for arXiv's community? Learn more about arXivLabs .

ScienceDaily

Talking politics with strangers isn't as awful as you'd expect, research suggests

Many of us avoid discussing politics with someone who holds an opposing viewpoint, assuming the exchange will turn nasty or awkward. But having those conversations is far more gratifying than we expect, a new research paper suggests.

Across a series of experiments involving hundreds of U.S. adults, a team of scientists found that individuals underestimate the social connection they can make with a stranger who disagrees with them. The findings are published in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

These low expectations may help to explain why people think those on the opposite side of the political spectrum have more extreme views than they actually do, behavioral scientists Kristina A. Wald (University of Pennsylvania), Michael Kardas (Oklahoma State University), and Nicholas Epley (University of Chicago) wrote in an article about their research.

"Mistakenly fearing a negative interaction may create misplaced partisan divides," they wrote, "not only keeping people from connecting with each other but also keeping people from learning about each other and from each other."

The experimenters found evidence, through experiments conducted online and in person, that people prefer to avoid hot-button issues, especially with people who disagree with them. People also tend to advise their friends and relatives to avoid such conversations.

But Wald, Kardas, and Epley believed people would find discussing their political differences to be a more positive experience than expected, at least partly because people fail to appreciate the extent to which conversations are informative and draw people closer together.

To test their theory, they asked nearly 200 participants in one experiment for their opinions on divisive political and religious topics, such as abortion and climate change. The researchers then divided the participants into pairs and assigned them to discuss one of these topics. Some participants were told in advance whether their partners agreed with them or not, but others entered the discussions unaware of their partners' views.

All the participants reported how positively or negatively they expected the conversation to be, then engaged in the discussion while being video recorded. Afterward, the participants rated their sentiments about the dialogue. Research assistants also viewed the videos of the conversations and evaluated them across several dimensions.

As predicted, the participants underestimated how positive their conversation experience would be, but this tendency was largest when they disagreed with their partner. Participants in this disagreement condition also underestimated the similarities in their opinions. Coders who watched the videos of these conversations confirmed that participants tended to stay on topic, and that the conversations were consistently positive whether the participants agreed or disagreed.

In another experiment, the researchers tested their hypothesis that people underestimate how the process of conversation itself -- actual back-and-forth dialogue -- connects people. To do so, they randomly assigned participants to discuss a divisive topic they agreed or disagreed on, but they also randomly assigned participants to either have a conversation about the topic in a dialogue format or to simply learn of their partners' beliefs on the topic in a monologue format. In the monologue format, each person separately recorded themselves talking about their opinion and then watched the other person's recording.

Overall, the participants underestimated how positive their interactions would be, especially when they disagreed with their partner, the researchers noted. But this tendency was especially strong when people actually had a conversation with their partner rather than simply learning of their beliefs in a monologue. The social forces in conversation that draw people together through back-and-forth dialogue are not only powerful, but they appear to be even more powerful than people expect.

The researchers cautioned that their experiments involved participants talking with strangers; the experiments did not reveal how disagreements unfold among family and friends. Still, they said their findings illustrate the benefits of talking and listening to others rather than typing and broadcasting in debates on social media.

Our reluctance to discuss our differences denies us some positive social interactions, the authors concluded.

"Misunderstanding the outcomes of a conversation," they wrote, "could lead people to avoid discussing disagreements more often, creating a misplaced barrier to learning, social connection, free inquiry, and free expression."

  • Relationships
  • Racial Issues
  • Social Psychology
  • STEM Education
  • Political Science
  • Surveillance
  • Poverty and Learning
  • Social movement
  • Intellectual giftedness
  • Social science
  • Social psychology
  • Double blind
  • Social cognition
  • Social inclusion

Story Source:

Materials provided by Association for Psychological Science . Note: Content may be edited for style and length.

Journal Reference :

  • Kristina A. Wald, Michael Kardas, Nicholas Epley. Misplaced Divides? Discussing Political Disagreement With Strangers Can Be Unexpectedly Positive . Psychological Science , 2024; DOI: 10.1177/09567976241230005

Cite This Page :

Explore More

  • RNA That Doesn't Age
  • 'Rainbow' Detected On an Exoplanet
  • Spears and Throwing Sticks 300,000 Years Old
  • High Carbon Impact of Tourism at Yellowstone
  • Extreme Starburst Galaxy
  • Asthma: Disease May Be Stoppable
  • Stellar Collisions and Zombie-Like Survivors
  • Tiny Robot Swarms Inspired by Herd Mentality
  • How the Brain Regulates Emotions
  • Evolution in Action? Nitrogen-Fixing Organelles

Trending Topics

Strange & offbeat.

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • 25 March 2024
  • Correction 27 March 2024

Weird new electron behaviour in stacked graphene thrills physicists

  • Dan Garisto

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

You have full access to this article via your institution.

Illustration showing four graphene layers.

Electrons in stacked sheets of staggered graphene collectively act as though they have fractional charges at ultralow temperatures. Credit: Ramon Andrade 3DCiencia/Science Photo Library

Minneapolis, Minnesota

Last May, a team led by physicists at the University of Washington in Seattle observed something peculiar. When the scientists ran an electrical current across two atom-thin sheets of molybdenum ditelluride (MoTe 2 ), the electrons acted in concert, like particles with fractional charges. Resistance measurements showed that, rather than the usual charge of –1, the electrons behaved similar to particles with charges of –2/3 or –3/5, for instance. What was truly odd was that the electrons did this entirely because of the innate properties of the material, without any external magnetic field coaxing them. The researchers published the results a few months later, in August 1 .

research essay on laughter

Strange topological materials are popping up everywhere physicists look

The same month, this phenomenon, known as the fractional quantum anomalous Hall effect (FQAHE), was also observed in a completely different material. Researchers led by Long Ju, a condensed-matter physicist at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT) in Cambridge, saw the effect when they sandwiched five layers of graphene between sheets of boron nitride. They published their results in February this year 2 — and physicists are still buzzing about it.

At the American Physical Society (APS) March Meeting, held in Minneapolis, Minnesota, from 3 to 8 March, Ju presented the team’s findings, which haven’t yet been replicated by other researchers. Attendees, including Raquel Queiroz, a theoretical physicist at Columbia University in New York City, said that they thought the results were convincing, but were scratching their heads over the discovery. “There is a lot we don’t understand,” Queiroz says. Figuring out the exact mechanism of the FQAHE in the layered graphene will be “a lot of work ahead of theorists”, she adds.

Although the FQAHE might have practical applications down the line — fractionally charged particles are a key requirement for a certain type of quantum computer — the findings are capturing physicists’ imagination because they are fundamentally new discoveries about how electrons behave.

“I don’t know anyone who’s not excited about this,” says Pablo Jarillo-Herrero, a condensed-matter physicist at MIT who was not involved with the studies. “I think the question is whether you’re so excited that you switch all your research and start working on it, or if you’re just very excited.”

Strange maths

Strange behaviour by electrons isn’t new.

In some materials, usually at temperatures near absolute zero, electrical resistance becomes quantized. Specifically, it’s the material’s transverse resistance that does this. (An electrical current encounters opposition to its flow in both the same direction as the current — called longitudinal resistance — and in the perpendicular direction — what’s called transverse resistance.)

Quantized ‘steps’ in the transverse resistance occur at integer multiples of electron charge: 1, 2, 3 and so on. These plateaus are the result of a strange phenomenon: the electrons maintain the same transverse resistance even as charge density increases. That’s a little like vehicles on a road moving at the same speed, even with more traffic. This is known as the quantum Hall effect.

In a different set of materials, with less disorder, the transverse resistance can even display plateaus at fractions of electron charge: 2/5, 3/7 and 4/9, for example. The plateaus take these values because the electrons collectively act like particles with fractional charges — hence the fractional quantum Hall effect (FQHE).

Key to both phenomena is a strong external magnetic field, which prevents electrons from crashing into each other and enables them to interact.

Four people standing next to a computer and a cryogenic measuring system.

(Left to right) Long Ju, Zhengguang Lu, Yuxuan Yao and Tonghang Han are all part of the team at MIT that demonstrated the fractional quantum anomalous Hall effect in layered graphene. Credit: Jixiang Yang

The FQHE, discovered in 1982, revealed the richness of electron behaviour. No longer could physicists think of electrons as single particles; in delicate quantum arrangements, the electrons could lose their individuality and act together to create fractionally charged particles. “I think people don’t appreciate how different [the fractional] is from the integer quantum Hall effect,” says Ashvin Vishwanath, a theoretical physicist at Harvard University in Cambridge. “It’s a new world.”

Over the next few decades, theoretical physicists came up with models to explain the FQHE and predict its effects. During their exploration, a tantalizing possibility appeared: perhaps a material could exhibit resistance plateaus without any external magnetic field. The effect, now dubbed the quantum anomalous Hall effect — ‘anomalous’, for the lack of a magnetic field — was finally observed in thin ferromagnetic films by a team at Tsinghua University in Beijing, in 2012 3 .

Carbon copy

Roughly a decade later, the University of Washington team reported the FQAHE for the first time 1 , in a specially designed 2D material: two sheets of MoTe 2 stacked on top of one another and offset by a twist.

This arrangement of MoTe 2 is known as a moiré material. Originally used to refer to a patterned textile, the term has been appropriated by physicists to describe the patterns in 2D materials created from atom-thin lattices when they are stacked and then twisted, or staggered atop one another. The slight offset between atoms in different layers of the material shifts the hills and valleys of its electric potential. And it effectively acts like a powerful magnetic field, taking the place of the one needed in the quantum Hall effect and the FQHE.

Xiaodong Xu, a condensed-matter physicist at the University of Washington, talked about the MoTe 2 discovery at the APS meeting. Theory hinted that the FQAHE would appear in the material at about a 1.4º twist angle. “We spent a year on it, and we didn’t see anything,” Xu told Nature .

Anomalous behaviour. Graphic showing the details of new moire material.

Source: Adapted from Ref. 2.

Then, the researchers tried a larger angle — a twist of about 4º. Immediately, they began seeing signs of the effect. Eventually, they measured the electrical resistance and spotted the signature plateaus of the FQAHE. Soon afterwards, a team led by researchers at Shanghai Jiao Tong University in China replicated the results 4 .

Meanwhile, at MIT, Ju was perfecting his technique, sandwiching graphene between layers of boron nitride. Similar to graphene, the sheets of boron nitride that Ju’s team used were a mesh of atoms linked together in a hexagonal pattern. The material’s lattice has a slightly different size from graphene’s; the mismatch creates a moiré pattern (see ‘Anomalous behaviour’).

Last month, Ju published a report 2 about seeing the characteristic plateaus. “It is a really amazing result,” Xu says. “I'm very happy to see there’s a second system.” Since then, Ju says, he’s also seen the effect when using four and six layers of graphene.

Both moiré systems have their pros and cons. MoTe 2 exhibited the effect at a few kelvin, as opposed to 0.1 kelvin for the layered graphene sandwich. (Low temperatures are required to minimize disorder in the systems.) But graphene is a cleaner and higher-quality material that is easier to measure. Experimentalists are now trying to replicate the results in graphene and find other materials that behave similarly.

Moiré than bargained for

Theorists are relatively comfortable with the MoTe 2 results, for which the FQAHE was partly predicted. But Ju’s layered graphene moiré was a shock to the community, and researchers are still struggling to explain how the effect happens. “There’s no universal consensus on what the correct theory is,” Vishwanath says. “But they all agree that it’s not the standard mechanism.” Vishwanath and his colleagues posted a preprint proposing a theory that the moiré pattern might not be that important to the FQAHE 5 .

research essay on laughter

Welcome anyons! Physicists find best evidence yet for long-sought 2D structures

One reason to doubt the importance of the moiré is the location of the electrons in the material: most of the activity is in the topmost layer of graphene, far away from the moiré pattern between the graphene and boron nitride at the bottom of the sandwich that is supposed to most strongly influence the electrons. But B. Andrei Bernevig, a theoretical physicist at Princeton University in New Jersey, and a co-author of another preprint proposing a mechanism for the FQAHE in the layered graphene 6 , urges caution about theory-based calculations, because they rely on currently unverified assumptions. He says that the moiré pattern probably matters, but less than it does in MoTe 2 .

For theorists, the uncertainty is exciting. “There are people who would say that everything has been seen in the quantum Hall effect,” Vishwanath says. But these experiments, especially the one using the layered graphene moiré, show that there are still more mysteries to uncover.

Nature 628 , 16-17 (2024)

doi: https://doi.org/10.1038/d41586-024-00832-z

Updates & Corrections

Correction 27 March 2024 : An earlier version of this story spelled researcher Tonghang Han’s name incorrectly in the photo caption.

Park, H. et al. Nature 622 , 74–79 (2023).

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Lu, Z. et al. Nature 626 , 759–764 (2024).

Chang, C.-Z. et al. Science 340 , 167–170 (2013).

Xu, F. et al. Phys. Rev. X 13 , 031037 (2023).

Article   Google Scholar  

Dong, J. et al. Preprint at arXiv https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2311.05568 (2023).

Kwan, Y. H. et al. Preprint at arXiv https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2312.11617 (2023).

Download references

Reprints and permissions

Related Articles

research essay on laughter

  • Materials science

Exclusive: official investigation reveals how superconductivity physicist faked blockbuster results

Exclusive: official investigation reveals how superconductivity physicist faked blockbuster results

News 06 APR 24

Phase-change memory via a phase-changeable self-confined nano-filament

Phase-change memory via a phase-changeable self-confined nano-filament

Article 03 APR 24

Shell buckling for programmable metafluids

Shell buckling for programmable metafluids

Heat flows enrich prebiotic building blocks and enhance their reactivity

Heat flows enrich prebiotic building blocks and enhance their reactivity

An optical tweezer array of ultracold polyatomic molecules

An optical tweezer array of ultracold polyatomic molecules

POSTDOCTORAL Fellow -- DEPARTMENT OF Surgery – BIDMC, Harvard Medical School

The Division of Urologic Surgery in the Department of Surgery at Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center and Harvard Medical School invites applicatio...

Boston, Massachusetts (US)

research essay on laughter

Director of Research

Applications are invited for the post of Director of Research at Cancer Institute (WIA), Chennai, India.

Chennai, Tamil Nadu (IN)

Cancer Institute (W.I.A)

research essay on laughter

Postdoctoral Fellow in Human Immunology (wet lab)

Join Atomic Lab in Boston as a postdoc in human immunology for universal flu vaccine project. Expertise in cytometry, cell sorting, scRNAseq.

Boston University Atomic Lab

research essay on laughter

Research Associate - Neuroscience and Respiratory Physiology

Houston, Texas (US)

Baylor College of Medicine (BCM)

research essay on laughter

Histology Laboratory Manager

research essay on laughter

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

Read our research on: Gun Policy | International Conflict | Election 2024

Regions & Countries

3. problems students are facing at public k-12 schools.

We asked teachers about how students are doing at their school. Overall, many teachers hold negative views about students’ academic performance and behavior.

  • 48% say the academic performance of most students at their school is fair or poor; a third say it’s good and only 17% say it’s excellent or very good.
  • 49% say students’ behavior at their school is fair or poor; 35% say it’s good and 13% rate it as excellent or very good.

Teachers in elementary, middle and high schools give similar answers when asked about students’ academic performance. But when it comes to students’ behavior, elementary and middle school teachers are more likely than high school teachers to say it’s fair or poor (51% and 54%, respectively, vs. 43%).

A horizontal stacked bar chart showing that many teachers hold negative views about students’ academic performance and behavior.

Teachers from high-poverty schools are more likely than those in medium- and low-poverty schools to say the academic performance and behavior of most students at their school are fair or poor.

The differences between high- and low-poverty schools are particularly striking. Most teachers from high-poverty schools say the academic performance (73%) and behavior (64%) of most students at their school are fair or poor. Much smaller shares of teachers from low-poverty schools say the same (27% for academic performance and 37% for behavior).

In turn, teachers from low-poverty schools are far more likely than those from high-poverty schools to say the academic performance and behavior of most students at their school are excellent or very good.

Lasting impact of the COVID-19 pandemic

A horizontal stacked bar chart showing that most teachers say the pandemic has had a lasting negative impact on students’ behavior, academic performance and emotional well-being.

Among those who have been teaching for at least a year, about eight-in-ten teachers say the lasting impact of the pandemic on students’ behavior, academic performance and emotional well-being has been very or somewhat negative. This includes about a third or more saying that the lasting impact has been very negative in each area.

Shares ranging from 11% to 15% of teachers say the pandemic has had no lasting impact on these aspects of students’ lives, or that the impact has been neither positive nor negative. Only about 5% say that the pandemic has had a positive lasting impact on these things.

A smaller majority of teachers (55%) say the pandemic has had a negative impact on the way parents interact with teachers, with 18% saying its lasting impact has been very negative.

These results are mostly consistent across teachers of different grade levels and school poverty levels.

Major problems at school

When we asked teachers about a range of problems that may affect students who attend their school, the following issues top the list:

  • Poverty (53% say this is a major problem at their school)
  • Chronic absenteeism – that is, students missing a substantial number of school days (49%)
  • Anxiety and depression (48%)

One-in-five say bullying is a major problem among students at their school. Smaller shares of teachers point to drug use (14%), school fights (12%), alcohol use (4%) and gangs (3%).

Differences by school level

A bar chart showing that high school teachers more likely to say chronic absenteeism, anxiety and depression are major problems.

Similar shares of teachers across grade levels say poverty is a major problem at their school, but other problems are more common in middle or high schools:

  • 61% of high school teachers say chronic absenteeism is a major problem at their school, compared with 43% of elementary school teachers and 46% of middle school teachers.
  • 69% of high school teachers and 57% of middle school teachers say anxiety and depression are a major problem, compared with 29% of elementary school teachers.
  • 34% of middle school teachers say bullying is a major problem, compared with 13% of elementary school teachers and 21% of high school teachers.

Not surprisingly, drug use, school fights, alcohol use and gangs are more likely to be viewed as major problems by secondary school teachers than by those teaching in elementary schools.

Differences by poverty level

A dot plot showing that majorities of teachers in medium- and high-poverty schools say chronic absenteeism is a major problem.

Teachers’ views on problems students face at their school also vary by school poverty level.

Majorities of teachers in high- and medium-poverty schools say chronic absenteeism is a major problem where they teach (66% and 58%, respectively). A much smaller share of teachers in low-poverty schools say this (34%).

Bullying, school fights and gangs are viewed as major problems by larger shares of teachers in high-poverty schools than in medium- and low-poverty schools.

When it comes to anxiety and depression, a slightly larger share of teachers in low-poverty schools (51%) than in high-poverty schools (44%) say these are a major problem among students where they teach.  

Discipline practices

A pie chart showing that a majority of teachers say discipline practices at their school are mild.

About two-thirds of teachers (66%) say that the current discipline practices at their school are very or somewhat mild – including 27% who say they’re very mild. Only 2% say the discipline practices at their school are very or somewhat harsh, while 31% say they are neither harsh nor mild.

We also asked teachers about the amount of influence different groups have when it comes to determining discipline practices at their school.

  • 67% say teachers themselves don’t have enough influence. Very few (2%) say teachers have too much influence, and 29% say their influence is about right.

A diverging bar chart showing that two-thirds of teachers say they don’t have enough influence over discipline practices at their school.

  • 31% of teachers say school administrators don’t have enough influence, 22% say they have too much, and 45% say their influence is about right.
  • On balance, teachers are more likely to say parents, their state government and the local school board have too much influence rather than not enough influence in determining discipline practices at their school. Still, substantial shares say these groups have about the right amount of influence.

Teachers from low- and medium-poverty schools (46% each) are more likely than those in high-poverty schools (36%) to say parents have too much influence over discipline practices.

In turn, teachers from high-poverty schools (34%) are more likely than those from low- and medium-poverty schools (17% and 18%, respectively) to say that parents don’t have enough influence.

Social Trends Monthly Newsletter

Sign up to to receive a monthly digest of the Center's latest research on the attitudes and behaviors of Americans in key realms of daily life

Report Materials

Table of contents, ‘back to school’ means anytime from late july to after labor day, depending on where in the u.s. you live, among many u.s. children, reading for fun has become less common, federal data shows, most european students learn english in school, for u.s. teens today, summer means more schooling and less leisure time than in the past, about one-in-six u.s. teachers work second jobs – and not just in the summer, most popular.

About Pew Research Center Pew Research Center is a nonpartisan fact tank that informs the public about the issues, attitudes and trends shaping the world. It conducts public opinion polling, demographic research, media content analysis and other empirical social science research. Pew Research Center does not take policy positions. It is a subsidiary of The Pew Charitable Trusts .

American Mathematical Society

Publications — Over 100 years of publishing excellence

  • Book Author Resources
  • Submit a Book Proposal
  • AMS Rights, Licensing, and Permissions
  • Open Math Notes
  • Frequently asked questions
  • Member Journals
  • Research Journals
  • Translation Journals
  • Distributed Journals
  • Open Access Journals
  • Guidelines and Policies
  • Journal Author Resources

Librarian Resources

  • eBook Collections
  • COUNTER Usage Statistics
  • My Subscriptions
  • Subscription Information
  • Licensing Information

Mathematical Reviews/MathSciNet®

  • MathSciNet ®
  • Reviewer Home
  • MathSciNet ® Subscriptions

Membership — Welcome to your membership center

Join the ams, renew your membership, give a membership, individual membership.

  • Member Benefits
  • Member Directory
  • Reciprocating Societies
  • Members in Developing Countries

Institutional Membership

  • Domestic Institutions
  • International Institutions
  • Two-Year Institutions
  • Graduate Student Chapter Program

Other Member Types

  • Corporate Memberships
  • Associate Memberships

Meetings & Conferences — Engage with colleagues and the latest research

National meetings.

  • Joint Mathematics Meetings
  • Upcoming JMMs
  • Previous JMMs
  • Special Lectures
  • Professional Enhancement Programs (PEPs)

Sectional Meetings

  • Upcoming Sectionals
  • Previous Sectionals
  • Presenting Papers
  • Hosting Sectionals

Other Meetings, Conferences & Workshops

  • Mathematics Research Communities
  • Education Mini-conference
  • International Meetings
  • Mathematics Calendar
  • Short Courses
  • Workshop for Department Chairs and Leaders

Meetings Resources

  • Suggest a Speaker
  • AMS Meetings Grants
  • Submitting Abstracts
  • Welcoming Environment Policy
  • MathSafe – supporting safe meetings

News & Outreach — Explore news, images, posters, and mathematical essays

News from the ams.

  • AMS News Releases
  • Feature Stories
  • Information for Journalists
  • In Memory Of

Math Voices

  • Feature Column
  • Math in the Media
  • Column on Teaching and Learning

Explorations

  • Recognizing Diverse Mathematicians
  • AMS Posters
  • Mathematics & Music
  • Mathematical Imagery
  • Mathematical Moments

Professional Programs — Resources and opportunities to further your mathematical pursuits

Professional development.

  • Employment Services
  • Mathjobs.org
  • BEGIN Career Initiative
  • Mathprograms.org
  • Mathematical Opportunities Database
  • Research Seminars

Institutional Information and Data

  • Annual Survey of the Mathematical and Statistical Sciences
  • CBMS Survey
  • Other Sources of Data
  • Directory of Institutions in the Mathematical Sciences
  • Professional Directory

Grants & Support

  • AMS-Simons Grants for PUI Faculty
  • Travel Grants
  • Fellowships & Scholarships
  • Epsilon Fund
  • Child Care Grants

Awards & Recognition

  • AMS Prizes & Awards
  • Fellows of the AMS

Education — Resources to support advanced mathematics teaching and learning

For students.

  • Information for Undergraduate and High School Students
  • Research Experiences for Undergraduates (REUs)
  • Considering Grad School
  • Find Grad Programs
  • Applying to Grad School
  • What do Mathematicians Do?

For Teachers

  • Teaching Online
  • Teaching Resources
  • Inclusive Classrooms
  • Assessing Student Learning
  • Education Webinars

For Department Leaders & Mentors

  • Information for Department Leaders
  • paraDIGMS (Diversity in Graduate Mathematical Sciences)

Government Relations — Advocating for the mathematical sciences

Elevating mathematics in congress.

  • Our Mission
  • Letters, Statements, & Legislation
  • Congressional Briefings

Legislative Priorities

  • Federal Issues of Concern
  • Federal Budget Process

Get Involved

  • Advocacy Resources
  • Take Action

DC-Based Fellowships

  • Congressional Fellowship
  • Mass Media Fellowship
  • Catalyzing Advocacy in Science & Engineering (CASE) Fellowship

Giving to the AMS — Your gifts make great things happen for mathematics   Make a Gift

What you can support.

  • The 2020 Fund
  • Next Generation Fund
  • Birman Fellowship for Women Scholars
  • JMM Child Care Grants
  • MathSciNet for Developing Countries

Create a Legacy

  • Make a Tribute Gift
  • Create a Permanent Fund
  • Establish a Prize, Award or Fellowship
  • Bequests and Charitable Estate Planning

Honoring Your Gift

  • Donor Stories
  • Donor Wall of Honor
  • Thomas S. Fiske Society
  • AMS Contributors Society
  • AMS Gardens

Giving Resources

  • AMS Development Committee
  • AMS Gift Acceptance Policy

About the AMS — Advancing research. Connecting the mathematics community.

Our organization.

  • Executive Staff
  • Equity, Diversity, & Inclusion
  • Jobs at AMS
  • Customer Service

Our Governance

  • Board of Trustees
  • Executive Committee

Governance Operations

  • Calendar of Meetings
  • Policy Statements & Guidelines

JOURNAL OF THE AMS

Bulletin of the American Mathematical Society

The Bulletin publishes expository articles on contemporary mathematical research, written in a way that gives insight to mathematicians who may not be experts in the particular topic. The Bulletin also publishes reviews of selected books in mathematics and short articles in the Mathematical Perspectives section, both by invitation only.

ISSN 1088-9485 (online) ISSN 0273-0979 (print)

The 2020 MCQ for Bulletin of the American Mathematical Society is 0.84 . What is MCQ? The Mathematical Citation Quotient (MCQ) measures journal impact by looking at citations over a five-year period. Subscribers to MathSciNet may click through for more detailed information.

  • Articles in press
  • Recently published
  • All issues : 1891 – Present

Contents of Volume 61, Number 2 HTML articles powered by AMS MathViewer View front and back matter from the print issue

IMAGES

  1. The Importance of Laughter Essay Example

    research essay on laughter

  2. Put Down Your Pills and Laugh

    research essay on laughter

  3. Laughter is the Best Medicine Essay for Students and Children in

    research essay on laughter

  4. essay on laughter is the best medicine/10 lines on laughter is the best

    research essay on laughter

  5. 😂 The power of laughter essay. The Power of Laughter « Kelly. 2019-01-27

    research essay on laughter

  6. Laughter is the Best Medicine Essay for Students and Children in

    research essay on laughter

VIDEO

  1. A Father Who Killed For His Son

  2. Research Essay Meeting

  3. Research Essay Writing PPWVIDEO pptx

  4. Research Essay

  5. Research essay: the spirit in art

  6. 10 lines essay on laughter is the Best Medicine/ an essay on laughter is the best medicine

COMMENTS

  1. Laughter as medicine: A systematic review and meta-analysis of interventional studies evaluating the impact of spontaneous laughter on cortisol levels

    Introduction. Laughter as an expression of humor has been recognized as a good medicine for centuries [], a concept consistent with human neurodevelopment.Specifically, the capacity for laughter in humans precedes the neural development of speech [] with neuroimaging studies suggesting a unique neural pathway for spontaneous laughter (i.e. genuine laughter) [] that is intuitive and subcortical.

  2. Humor, laughter, learning, and health! A brief review

    Human emotions, such as anxiety, depression, fear, joy, and laughter, profoundly impact psychological and physiological processes. These emotions form a set of basic, evolved functions that are shared by all humans ( 51 ). Health care providers and educators may take advantage of powerful emotions to improve health and enhance learning.

  3. Impact of Laughter on Health, Happiness and Wellbeing

    A significant amount of research that examines the impact of laughter on health concludes that it brings about physical, emotional, and social benefits (Bennett et al., 2014; Mora-Ripoll & Ubal-López, 2011).However, although many studies purport to examine laughter, often the variable under investigation is laughter created as a consequence of the participant being exposed to something ...

  4. Introduction to the Special Issue of the Scientific Study of Laughter

    In his pioneering work, The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, Darwin (1872/1965) argued that laughter was the outward expression of mirth. In the 150 years since the publication of this book, research has revealed that laughter is substantially more complex, nuanced, and socially important than Darwin theorized. The current special issue highlights recent discoveries regarding the ...

  5. The different shades of laughter: when do we laugh and when do we mimic

    1. Introduction. Laughter is a pervasive and highly contagious behaviour that is typically shown in enjoyable and friendly interactions [].Similar to non-human play vocalizations, it fosters social bonding, as it is linked to positive emotions such as joy that function to signal positive intents [].Yet, laughter can also be malicious, especially when related to ridicule and schadenfreude ...

  6. What Makes Things Funny? An Integrative Review of the Antecedents of

    Our review helps organize a disconnected literature, dispel popular but inaccurate ideas, offers a framework for future research, and helps answer three long-standing questions about humor: what conditions predict laughter and amusement, what are the adaptive benefits of humor, and why do different people think vastly different things are humorous?

  7. Is Laughter Really the Best Medicine? Reflecting on a Mental Health

    Perhaps because of the universality of laughter (Wood and Niedenthal 2018), these studies and applications resonate with the public.The Australian Laughie Challenge 2022 launched by Merv Neal, the Head of Laughter Yoga Australia and an experienced laughter professional, was inspired by the first author's Laughie research (Gonot-Schoupinsky and Garip 2019).

  8. Why do we laugh? New study considers possible evolutionary reasons

    I looked at research papers on theories of humour that provided significant information on three areas: the physical features of laughter, the brain centres related to producing laughter, and the ...

  9. Laughing is good for your mind and your body

    Laughter has value across the whole lifespan. Steve Prezant/The Image Bank via Getty Images. Deep knowledge, daily. Some tips on how to get started with laughing yoga. Whether in the form of a ...

  10. The science of laughter and why it's good for us

    This can give you a pain-killing sensation. As you laugh, it lowers your adrenaline levels and over a longer time frame your levels of the stress hormone cortisol, she explained. As such, laughter ...

  11. The Science of Laughter

    In 3,745 ads placed on April 28, 1996 in eight papers from the Baltimore Sun to the San Diego Union-Tribune, females were 62% more likely to mention laughter in their ads, and women were more ...

  12. The Health Benefits of Laughter

    Research has shown that the health benefits of laughter are far-ranging. Studies so far have shown that laughter can help relieve pain, bring greater happiness, and even increase immunity. Positive psychology names the propensity for laughter and sense of humor as one of the 24 main signature strengths one can possess.  

  13. Laughing Your Way to Health and Joy

    Current scientific research at Loma Linda University's School of Allied Health and Medicine has shown that laughter helps the brain regulate the ... 350+ Scientific Research Papers on Laughter

  14. 4 Health Benefits of Laughing

    4. Heart health. Early research suggests laughter can decrease stress hormones, reduce artery inflammation and increase HDL, the "good" cholesterol. Here are three ways in which laughter can ...

  15. The Science of Humor Is No Laughing Matter

    Why April 1st became a holiday devoted to pranks and laughter remains a mystery, although some historians trace it back to the Roman holiday of Hilaria. Humans start developing a sense of humor as early as 6 weeks old, when babies begin to laugh and smile in response to stimuli. Laughter is universal across human cultures and even exists in ...

  16. Laughter is the Best Medicine

    Laughter is good for your health. Laughter relaxes the whole body. A good, hearty laugh relieves physical tension and stress, leaving your muscles relaxed for up to 45 minutes after. Laughter boosts the immune system. Laughter decreases stress hormones and increases immune cells and infection-fighting antibodies, thus improving your resistance ...

  17. 350+ Scientific Research Papers on Laughter

    Adelswärd, Viveka, and Britt-Marie Öberg. "The Function of Laughter and Joking in Negotiating Activities." HUMOR: International Journal of Humor Research 11.4 (1998): 411-430.

  18. The Effect of Laughter Yoga on the Quality of Life of Elderly Nursing

    The experimental group received an interventional program consisting of Madan Kataria Laughter Therapy, showing comic films, and reading comic books for one month, over 12 sessions of 100 minutes. After the end of laughter intervention, the elderly of both groups responded to the elderly quality of life questionnaire (LIPAD).

  19. [2403.20329] ReALM: Reference Resolution As Language Modeling

    ReALM: Reference Resolution As Language Modeling. Reference resolution is an important problem, one that is essential to understand and successfully handle context of different kinds. This context includes both previous turns and context that pertains to non-conversational entities, such as entities on the user's screen or those running in the ...

  20. Mapping the Increasing Use of LLMs in Scientific Papers

    In comparison, Mathematics papers and the Nature portfolio showed the least LLM modification (up to 6.3%). Moreover, at an aggregate level, our analysis reveals that higher levels of LLM-modification are associated with papers whose first authors post preprints more frequently, papers in more crowded research areas, and papers of shorter lengths.

  21. Talking politics with strangers isn't as awful as you'd expect

    Talking politics with strangers isn't as awful as you'd expect, research suggests. ScienceDaily . Retrieved April 4, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com / releases / 2024 / 04 / 240403171027.htm

  22. Weird new electron behaviour in stacked graphene thrills physicists

    Researchers led by Long Ju, a condensed-matter physicist at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT) in Cambridge, saw the effect when they sandwiched five layers of graphene between sheets ...

  23. Problems students are facing at public K-12 schools

    Major problems at school. When we asked teachers about a range of problems that may affect students who attend their school, the following issues top the list: Poverty (53% say this is a major problem at their school) Chronic absenteeism - that is, students missing a substantial number of school days (49%) Anxiety and depression (48%) One-in ...

  24. AMS :: Bull. Amer. Math. Soc. -- Volume 61, Number 2

    The Bulletin publishes expository articles on contemporary mathematical research, written in a way that gives insight to mathematicians who may not be experts in the particular topic. The Bulletin also publishes reviews of selected books in mathematics and short articles in the Mathematical Perspectives section, both by invitation only.